Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Male urine signals social rank in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus)

Abstract

Background

The urine of freshwater fish species investigated so far acts as a vehicle for reproductive pheromones affecting the behaviour and physiology of the opposite sex. However, the role of urinary pheromones in intra-sexual competition has received less attention. This is particularly relevant in lek-breeding species, such as the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus), where males establish dominance hierarchies and there is the possibility for chemical communication in the modulation of aggression among males. To investigate whether males use urine during aggressive interactions, we measured urination frequency of dye-injected males during paired interactions between size-matched males. Furthermore, we assessed urinary volume stored in the bladder of males in a stable social hierarchy and the olfactory potency of their urine by recording of the electro-olfactogram.

Results

Males released urine in pulses of short duration (about one second) and markedly increased urination frequency during aggressive behaviour, but did not release urine whilst submissive. In the stable hierarchy, subordinate males stored less urine than males of higher social rank; the olfactory potency of the urine was positively correlated with the rank of the male donor.

Conclusion

Dominant males store urine and use it as a vehicle for odorants actively released during aggressive disputes. The olfactory potency of the urine is positively correlated with the social status of the male. We suggest that males actively advertise their dominant status through urinary odorants which may act as a 'dominance' pheromone to modulate aggression in rivals, thereby contributing to social stability within the lek.

Background

Pheromones are involved in the reproductive and non-reproductive (migration, parent-young interactions, schooling and related social behaviours) behaviours of fish (reviewed in [1, 2]). In general, pheromones are released into the water via the urine, gills, skin or faeces, and they trigger adaptive physiological and behavioural responses in conspecifics. Reproductive hormones (steroids and prostaglandins or their metabolites) released into the water can be employed as hormonal pheromones, reflecting the reproductive status of the donor and affecting the reproductive physiology and behaviour of receivers [2]. Most studies, however, have focused on the role of pheromones in regulating the interaction between the sexes and few studies have dealt with the roles for putative pheromones in the regulation of competitive interactions within sexes. In goldfish (Carassius auratus), which have the best-understood sex pheromone system among teleosts [2, 3], hormonal pheromones from females function primarily as timing cues to synchronize male and female spawning. In addition, it has been shown that male goldfish respond strongly to the presence of male conspecifics. They increase sperm stores either in response to nearby males with higher levels of the sex steroid hormone 17,20β-dihydroxy-4-pregnen-3-one, or in response to isolation from a group of conspecific males in a basal endocrine condition [4, 5]. The latter is probably because of the action of inhibitory male steroidal cues dominated by androstenedione [2], which is released in large amounts through the gills of sexually active males and has been suggested to be a male pheromone regulating competitive interactions among males [6]. In zebrafish (Danio rerio), male pheromones stimulate female reproduction and increase the quality and viability of eggs. However, putative pheromones from conspecific females inhibit female spawning[7].

Although the interrelationships between social behaviour (not directly involved in reproduction) and physiology have been extensively studied in several teleost species [811], the role of pheromones in the regulation of social behaviour has received little attention. In species where males aggregate in breeding arenas (lek breeders) and dominance hierarchies are established, there is a possibility for chemical communication and a role for pheromones in the modulation of aggressive interactions among males. The Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus) is a good model candidate to test this hypothesis because males practice continuous lekking [12], without leaving their territories to forage, during the entire breeding period.

O. mossambicus is a maternal mouth-brooder cichlid endemic to the lakes and rivers of the east coast of Africa [13], in which the males form dense aggregations over sandy substrates during the breeding season [13, 14]. Territorial males adopt a characteristic black colouration and defend small territories centred on pits (nests) that they dig in the sand. Visiting females spawn in these territories, but brood the fertilized eggs and subsequent fry away from the leks [13, 1517]. The social behaviour of this species has received considerable attention, as has the causal processes underlying colouration patterns (see [1823] and references therein). In captivity, males form dominance hierarchies which are stable for several days [18, 2325]. The largest alpha males receive more visits by females [25] and the majority of spawnings [18], and the dominance structure is positively correlated with urinary sex hormone levels [19, 23]. During hierarchy formation, the initial assessment includes asymmetrical behavioural displays that can escalate to higher levels of symmetrical aggression, including mouth-to-mouth fighting. Once the hierarchy is established, the overall level of aggression decreases and asymmetrical displays are the most common agonistic interactions observed. A further decrease in aggression occurs after the addition of females to the group [23]. During paired interactions 'resident' males dramatically increase their urination frequency in the presence of 'intruder' males [26, 27]. Since urine from males is a vehicle of odorants, at least for conspecific females [28], it is possible that dominant/territorial males actively advertise their aggressive motivation and social status to other males using urinary odorants, which may act as male-male pheromones, in addition to visual displays. The goal of the current study was to determine (i) whether the male urination pattern is linked to aggressive behaviour and (ii) whether the olfactory potency of male urine on conspecific males is related with the social status of the male donor.

Results

Urination and male behaviour

During social isolation male tilapia urinated at very low frequency (on average one pulse every 10 min) and expelled urine in short-duration pulses (less than 2 s). Resident males markedly increased their urination frequency during the first 5 min following the introduction of an intruder. During the subsequent 5 min, the urination frequency decreased and after 10 min resident males stopped releasing urine (Figure 1). All four resident males reacted aggressively soon after the intruder male was introduced into the tank; an increase of urination frequency occurred when resident males initiated aggressive displays. In three cases, resident males eventually stopped releasing urine pulses after the aggressive interaction escalated to mouth-to-mouth fighting. In one case, the intruder male was never aggressive and the resident male eventually stopped the release of urine pulses. Urine release was never observed in resident males whilst being not aggressive. The mean urine pulse duration was 1.4 ± 0.1 s (mean ± SEM, n = 126 pulses) and, apparently, the mean pulse duration per male did not change between social isolation (1.4 ± 0.2 s, n = 4) and during interaction with the intruder male (1.5 ± 0.3 s), indicating that the total urine volume released increased with urination frequency. No obvious changes in the release of faeces or intestinal fluids were noted. The trend indicating a link between increased urination and the aggressive behaviour of the sending male (without obvious changes in duration of urine pulses or release of faeces or intestinal fluids) was also observed when four male pairs were repeatedly used (after at least a one-week interval). The same male was used four times and in one-half of the replicates was either a dye-injected resident or a dye-injected intruder. However, given that our experimental design did not allow control over possible memory effects of the previous interaction on subsequent male behaviour and urination frequency, we have not included these data.

Figure 1
figure 1

Urination frequency of male tilapia. Frequency of urination (pulses per minute every 5 min; mean ± SEM; n = 4) by dye-injected resident males in social isolation (open horizontal bar) followed by the presence of a saline-injected intruder male of similar size (dark horizontal bar).

In the experiment with neighbour males, the mean urination frequency increased tenfold when the two males came into contact (social isolation: 0.04 ± 0.02 pulses.min-1; with contact: 0.4 ± 0.1 pulses.min-1, n = 16, Student's t test for paired samples, t 15 = 7.43, p < 0.001; Figure 2A). The urine volume collected from each male at the end of each observation varied between 0 and 1.6 ml; males from which no urine was collected had high urination frequency during the observation period. In every observation, both males increased their urination frequency, but the level and timing of the increase varied within and between replicates and were related to the level of aggression (Figure 2B provides three examples). In all cases, there were periods of symmetrical aggression during which both males increased urination frequency. Urine release was not observed during submissive behaviours or whenever one male became submissive to its opponent (50% of the replicates). The dominant male also stopped urinating during its aggressive displays when the opponent male became submissive, despite still having urine in the bladder (this was checked at the end of the experiment). Overall, urination frequency was significantly higher during aggressive displays and symmetrical high aggression than during non-aggressive behaviours (Figure 3).

Figure 2
figure 2

Urination frequency and behaviour of two neighbouring tilapia males. (A) Frequency of urination (pulses per minute every 5 min; mean ± SEM; n = 16) by males in social isolation (open horizontal bar) followed by contact with a neighbour male (dark horizontal bar). (B) Examples (1–3) of behaviour (submissive: white; not aggressive: light grey; aggressive displays: dark grey; highly aggressive: black) and release of urine pulses (urination), of around 1 s, during 45 min of interaction between two territorial male tilapia (a) and (b). In (1), male (a) increased its urination frequency 25 min after coming into contact with male (b) and initiated aggressive displays which escalated to symmetrical high aggression (circle fight); in turn, male (b) changed from submissive, not urinating, to aggressive displays and its urination frequency increased as the agonistic interaction escalated to high symmetrical aggression. In (2), both males increased their urination frequency within 10–15 min when both initiated aggressive displays which further escalated to high symmetrical aggression; although the two males maintained this level of aggression throughout the observation period, their urination frequency dropped significantly. In (3), we see the only case of high urination frequency observed during non-aggressive behaviours, before aggressive behaviour escalation, during which the frequency of urination was also high; male (a) became submissive within a few minutes after the symmetrical high aggression and stopped urinating, whereas male (b) continued with aggressive displays and stopped urine release as well.

Figure 3
figure 3

Urination and behaviour of neighbouring tilapia males. Urination frequency (mean ± SEM, n = 16) during non-aggressive behaviour, aggressive displays and high aggression. Different letters over the bars indicate significant differences (repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), F 1,15 = 125.6, p < 0.001 followed by least significant difference (LSD) pairwise comparisons, p < 0.05).

Social hierarchies, urine volume and olfactory potency of male urine

In the community tank, five males had a subordinate status (daily dominance index (Di): 0–0.21) and seldom or never exhibited dominant behaviours. The other seven males were of higher social rank (Di ≥ 0.5); four of the high social rank males were the most dominant males (Di: 0.73–0.91), occupied a fixed position in the tank and seldom or never showed submissive behaviours. Size and growth rate was not different between subordinate males (standard length (SL) = 148.2 ± 1.7 mm; body weight (BW) = 96.8 ± 3.6 g; SL growth = 0.18 ± 0.04 mm.day-1; BW growth = 0.29 ± 0.13 g.day-1) and males of higher social rank (SL = 152.9 ± 2.0 mm, t 10 = 1.697, p = 1.121; BW = 109.1 ± 5.3 g, t 10 = 1.771, p = 1.107; SL growth = 0.18 ± 0.05 mm.day-1, t 10 = 0.037, p = 0.971; BW growth = 0.28 ± 0.14 g.day-1, t 10 = 0.055, p = 0.958). However, the mean urine volume collected from subordinate males was lower than that from males of higher social rank (Figure 4A). Also, the mean electro-olfactogram (EOG) amplitude evoked by urine from subordinate males was significantly smaller than that elicited by urine samples from males of higher social rank (Figure 4B).

Figure 4
figure 4

Urine volume, urine olfactory potency and dominance index of donor male tilapia in a community tank. (A) Scatter plot of urine volume (mean ± SEM, n = 5 days) collected from males (n = 12) and their Di (mean ± SEM, n = 5 days) during 5 days in a community tank with females. The mean urine volume collected from the five subordinate males (Di between 0.0 and 0.21) was significantly smaller (0.39 ± 0.09 ml) than that collected from males of higher social rank (0.83 ± 0.06 ml; Student's t test, t 10 = 4.330, p < 0.005). (B) Scatter plot of EOG amplitudes (mean ± SEM, n = 30) elicited by male urine (1:104 v/v in water) and the dominance index (mean ± SEM, n = 5 days) of donor males (n = 12). The urine of subordinate males evoked significantly smaller normalized EOG responses (0.87 ± 0.14) than that of higher rank males (1.92 ± 0.12; t 10 = 5.715, p < 0.001).

Discussion

The current study provides strong empirical evidence for a close link between male aggression and increased urination in male tilapia. This behaviour is different from that reported in earlier studies [26, 29], in which males increased their urination frequency when a pre-ovulatory female was introduced into their tank. A major difference in the response to pre-ovulatory females, however, is that the increase of urination frequency was not linked to specific behavioural displays (e.g. courtship) whereas in the current study the increase of urination frequency occurred together with aggressive behaviours. Pre-ovulatory female tilapia release specific odorants into the water and males base their increase in urination frequency on this olfactory information which does not affect the intensity of courtship behaviours [29]. It is not known whether odorants from conspecific males may modulate the behaviour and/or urination frequency as suggested for male-female interactions.

In all cases when both males released urine during symmetrical aggression without the emergence of a loser or winner, high urination frequency occurred immediately before the aggression or soon after the start of aggressive displays. Also, in all cases when a male soon became submissive, the aggressive male did not release urine. When a male became submissive after engaging in symmetrical aggression, the dominant male stopped urinating. As the urine is a vehicle of potent odorants, especially urine from males of higher social rank, and urine was never released during submissive behaviours, these results raise the possibility that aggressive territorial males actively release chemical information to rival males. Urinary odorants, alone or in a mixture with possible and additional odorants from other sources (e.g. skin mucous, faeces and/or gills), may reinforce information about the aggressive motivation of the sender conveyed through other sensory channels such as vision (sound is not emitted during aggressive behaviour [30]). However, the precise effect that urinary odorants may have on the receiver is not yet clear. Possibly, depending on its motivational state and the opponent's behaviour, a male may give up the dispute; the decision-making process could be influenced by chemical information in its opponent's urine. Only manipulative experiments will show the effect of urine on receiver males and whether and how urine odorants are necessary to exert a dominant status.

The identities of the urinary odorants are as yet unknown. In the tilapia, the concentration of urinary steroids depends on social context. Testosterone (T), 11-ketotestosterone (11KT), 17,20β-dihydroxy-4-pregnen-3-one (17,20β-P) and 17,20α-dihydroxy-4-pregnen-3-one (17,20α-P) are positively correlated with dominance [19]. However, none of these steroids evoke significant EOG responses even at concentrations as high as 10-6 M (see [28]). Some empirical evidence suggests that, at least in some species, gonadal androgens may function as male-derived pheromones that denote maturational state or gender. In goldfish, sexually active males release, via the gills, relatively large amounts of androstenedione [6] which stimulates pushing behaviour (a component of male-male competition) [31] and has been suggested to act as a male pheromone (as part of a blend of steroids dominated by androgens) in the regulation of competitive interactions among males [2, 6, 31]. Mature male silver barbs, Punctius schwanenfeldi, detect 11KT at threshold concentrations of about 10-11 M (see [32]). In the lek-breeding African cichlid, Haplochromis burtoni, both sexes appear to detect only conjugated (sulphate or glucuronide) sex steroids (at thresholds of 10-11 M) of which six were androgens including testosterone-sulphate (T-17S) [33]. Although the olfactory system of the Mozambique tilapia is not sensitive to T, T-17S or 11-KT [28], other androgen metabolites in male urine, which may also correlate with dominance, could act as odorants. However, a recent study using liquid chromatography linked to mass spectrometry and recording of EOG showed that female tilapia detect a single compound in male urine, suggested to be a sulphated amino-sterol (Barata et al., unpublished). This odorant is present at higher concentrations in the urine of dominant males than in subordinate males. Whether this is the same and only odorant in male urine that is detected by conspecific males is under investigation.

The current study shows that dominant males actively release chemical information by increased urination during aggression and that their urine has higher olfactory potency than that of subordinate males. Although these results support a role for urinary odorants in advertising aggressive motivation and dominance, other odorants are released to the water via intestinal fluids and, possibly, the gills and skin [28]. These may also reflect male social status and have a role in mediating aggressive interactions among males. However, no obvious changes were seen in the rate of release of intestinal fluids. Nevertheless, whether urinary odorants are the most important odorants or whether other odorants released via other routes are also involved in male-male interactions needs further investigation.

In contrast, females release urine in shorter pulses, but at higher frequency, than males. Furthermore, this is apparently unaffected by the presence of a sexually active male [26]. Since a territorial male of 100 g can accumulate up to 2 ml of urine in the urinary bladder [26], storage of urinary odorants for release in the appropriate social context seems to be a male-specific characteristic for this species. On the other hand, increase of urine release in association with visual displays could represent a simple modification of an extant behaviour to convey and amplify chemical information that adds to visual displays, and to sound, in defined social contexts, i.e. territorial disputes or courtship. As the duration of pulses did not change with increased urination frequency, the total volume of urine released should also increase. In addition, a pulsed signal creates intermittent stimulation and is less likely to cause sensory adaptation in the receiver, especially in the close proximity that these social interactions occur.

Urine volume collected from dominant males was consistently higher than that collected from subordinate males. Although it could be argued that the method used to collect urine does not allow for accurate estimates of urine volume in the bladder, the fact that it was employed consistently by the same person and that care was taken to randomize the order by which males were sampled suggest that our measurements are valid for the relative comparison between males of different social rank. Furthermore, difference in stored urine volume between dominant and subordinate males has been observed in two other social groups (with the same number of fish in similar tanks) sampled for urine in the same way (Barata et al., unpublished). Taken together, these results suggest that social dominance may require controlled release of urine during the appropriate social interactions and that this ability is less apparent in subordinate males and females.

Freshwater fish are hyperosmotic to their environment and need to expel excess water to maintain osmotic balance and, therefore, tend to produce large volumes of dilute urine. The outlet through the urogenital papilla is guarded by one or more sphincters, and in freshwater rainbow trout the urine can be stored in the bladder for about 25 min before release (periodic pulses at 20–30 min intervals) [34]; this storage period seems to allow the bladder epithelium to supplement the kidney in reabsorption of NaCl [34, 35]. Social status may affect the capacity/need to control urine storage and release from the urinary bladder; subordinate males may produce less urine (lower glomerular filtration rate), dominant and subordinate males may produce similar volumes of urine but subordinates retain lower volume in the bladder, or both urine flow rate and storage capacity are different between subordinate and higher rank males. To the best of our knowledge, these questions have not yet been addressed in the tilapia. In rainbow trout, urine flow rates and plasma cortisol concentrations are higher in subordinate fish but there is no difference in glomerular filtration rate between dominant and subordinates [36]. By extension, if social subordination induces increased urine flow in tilapia, then the larger urine volume collected from dominants could be explained by an active and precise control of release of urine stored by dominant males.

The physiological mechanisms that control the urination behaviour of fish have not been studied. In mice, however, changes in androgen levels affect the urinary tract and urination behaviour. Testosterone induces increased bladder muscular mass and affects the urination pattern of both males and females [37]; male mice deposit urine around territorial boundaries, through scent-marking behaviour, to advertise territorial dominance and their competitive ability to potential mates and competitors, also allowing individual recognition [38, 39]. In male tilapia, androgen levels (11KT) are good predictors of dominance [19], but their effect in urinary tract physiology or urination behaviour is not known. Mature masu salmon show morphological changes in the urinary tract suggesting a functional transition in the kidney related to the release of sex pheromones in both sexes [40, 41]. Further investigation is necessary to determine which physiological mechanisms (e.g. urinary bladder morphology and neuroendocrine control of urine storage and release) explain the different urination behaviour between subordinate and dominant males, and how a shift in social status may affect such mechanisms in the tilapia.

Urinary odorants may be involved in the emergence of social hierarchy at the initial stage of lek formation and, thereafter, in the maintenance of social stability as previously described [18, 2325]. Once a male has gained a high social status, maintaining dominance may result, at least in part, from its ability to respond to challenges from other males with urine release that reflect its physiological state. On the other hand, olfactory input during previous disputes may affect the pattern of subsequent social interactions. In the tilapia, winning or losing an encounter with another male modifies subsequent sound production and courtship [42]. However, it is not yet known whether urinary odorants play a role in this effect or if previous fighting experience affects the urination behaviour.

In general, fighting experience influences the outcome of a later contest (known as the winner and loser effects) and may mediate the formation of social hierarchies where individual recognition might reduce aggression among group members (reviewed in [43]). In the closely related Nile tilapia, which has a similar reproductive strategy, fish holding-water decreased aggression within pairs of juvenile fish and this was more accentuated when the fish pairs (of undetermined sex) were from the same original group [44]. Undoubtedly, individual recognition is an important stabilizing factor for social hierarchies in permanent or semi-permanent social groups [45, 46] and fish are capable of individual recognition through olfaction [47] and through associative learning with olfactory cues [48]. In the cichlid, Astatotilapia burtoni, which have reproductive and social systems similar to those of tilapia, males seem to build spatial and featural representations related to rival abilities which they can use to infer social rank [49]. It is possible that the build up of memory from such visual information is aided by olfactory input allowing further individual recognition and the assessment of a rival's social rank. Based on our study, we suggest that urinary odorants may play a role in the above-mentioned aspects of male tilapia social behaviour, although further investigation is required.

The release of urine signals during aggressive interactions may not be unique to male tilapia and may be found in fish that aggregate temporarily and/or fight for territory, having high levels of intra-sexual competition. To the best of our knowledge, however, this has not been investigated, but it is certainly not unique to fishes. In some crustaceans, increased release of urine occurs during aggressive interactions between males [5052], suggesting that linking urine signals to aggressive behaviour may have evolved independently in different taxa of aquatic animals.

Conclusion

Dominant males store more urine in the bladder than subordinates. Urine is actively released when the male is involved in aggressive disputes. The olfactory potency of urine from dominant males is greater than that from subordinates. We suggest that urine is a vehicle for male-pheromones which may aid in exerting social dominance.

Methods

Experimental animals

Mozambique tilapia were obtained from a brood-stock maintained at the University of Algarve (Faro, Portugal). For several weeks before use in experiments, groups of fish (one male and three to four females) were housed in re-circulating aquaria (93 × 60 × 55 cm) with sand substrate, containing 200 l de-chlorinated tapwater at 27°C, photoperiod 12 L : 12 D. Fish were fed twice a day with a commercial cichlid food (Nutrafin basix®; Rolf C. Hagen, Inc., Montreal, Canada). Spawning occurred in all fish groups, but the fertilized eggs were removed and incubated elsewhere.

Visualization of urination

Urination was visualized as described previously [26], using a method based on that of Appelt and Sorensen [53]. Males were lightly anaesthetized by immersion in iced water for 2–3 min and injected in the dorsal musculature with 100 μl of 100 mg.ml-1 patent blue violet (Sigma) in 0.9% NaCl. The fish were placed back in their tank, allowed to recover and experiments started after the first visible pulse of blue urine. Although fluids released through faeces are also coloured, urine seen was as a blue plume jetting from the urogenital opening in the genital papilla and was clearly discernible from release of faeces or fluids from the anus.

Urination and behaviour in male-male interactions

A first set of experiments was carried out to assess urination frequency and duration in social isolation and during aggressive interactions. The resident-intruder paradigm was employed with four pairs of size-matched territorial males (SL = 125.6 ± 3.6 mm, mean ± SEM, n = 8; BW = 62.3 ± 4.4 g; maximum SL and BW difference between two males was 1 mm and 3.9 g, respectively). The two males were taken from their family tanks and isolated in glass tanks (80 × 35 × 45 cm, 50 l, 27°C, aerated) with sand substrate for 24 h prior to the experiment. The resident male was injected with the dye and the 'intruder' male was injected with saline (0.9% NaCl). After 45 min in social isolation, the resident male received the intruder into its tank and the behaviour of both fish was recorded on video for 45 min (Panasonic SX60 super VHS). Urine release and duration of each urine pulse was recorded by the observer using a handheld computer (Psion Organizer LZ64) programmed for timing start and end of each urine pulse through the stroke of two keys. Immediately after each replicate, the males were lightly anaesthetized by immersion in iced water for 2–3 min and urine was sampled by gently squeezing the abdomen immediately above and anterior to the genital papilla. Squeezing of this abdominal area caused erection of the genital papilla and a jet of urine was collected. Urine volume was measured by weighing. Each male pair was used only once and, in each replicate, only the 'resident' male was injected with the dye.

In the second set of experiments, the neighbouring males paradigm was used. Eight pairs of size-matched males (SL = 136.6 ± 3.2 mm; BW = 76.8 ± 5.0 g; maximum SL and BW difference between two males was 1 mm and 4.1 g, respectively) were chosen. Two males were injected with the dye and placed in either side of a glass tank (similar to the method described above), separated from each other by a sliding opaque partition in the middle which prevented visual or chemical contact between the two sides. After recovery, the males were filmed for 30 min. The partition was then lifted and filming continued for another 45 min. The frequency of urine release was recorded by the observer using the handheld computer programmed for timing urine release from each male. Immediately after each replicate, the males were sampled for urine and returned to their original family tanks.

The Observer PC software V4.0 or V5.0 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands) was used to score the frequency and duration of male behaviours recorded in the videos. The behaviours were grouped into the following behaviour classes: non-aggressive behaviours (e.g. swimming), submissive behaviours (escape from an aggressive opponent or submission postures), aggressive displays and symmetrical high aggression (biting or chasing each other in a circle interrupted by mouth-fighting). These behaviours are described in detail by Baerends and Baerends van Roon [16] and Oliveira [22].

Social hierarchies in a community tank

To establish social hierarchies and obtain urine samples from dominant and subordinate males for subsequent assessment of olfactory potency on males (see below), 12 males and 14 females (all sexually mature) were housed for 23 days in a community white plastic tank (128 × 110 × 50 cm) containing de-chlorinated tap-water (500 l) at 25–27°C, photoperiod 12 L : 12 D, and feeding was once a day with commercial cichlid food. The males were tagged with coloured plastic labels (T-Bar extra small anchor, FF-94, FLOY TAG Inc., Seattle, WA) attached to the muscle near the dorsal fin and their behaviour was systematically observed during three observation sessions per day (morning, midday and afternoon) over 5 days starting 18 days after group formation. In every observation session, the group was observed continuously for 60 min and the frequency of submissive behaviours (escape from an aggressive opponent or submission postures without dark colouration), dominant behaviours (aggressive displays or nest-digging) and courtship displays were recorded for each male. Although the community tank had no sand, some males exhibited normal nest-digging behaviour by nipping the bottom. After the afternoon observations, each male was caught with a net, slightly anaesthetized (50 mg.l-1 3-aminobenzoic acid ethyl ester, MS222) and sampled for urine as described above. The fish were allowed to recover and placed back in the tank. The urine from each male was stored at -20°C until use for the assessment of olfactory potency (see below). To quantify the males' social rank, Di was calculated as the ratio between the summed frequency of all dominant behaviours (including courtship) and the summed frequency of dominant and submissive behaviours. The SL and BW of the males were measured at the start of the social group formation (SL = 146.8 ± 1.6 mm, BW = 97.3 ± 3.8 g) and at the end of the third week after group formation (SL = 150.9 ± 1.5 mm, BW = 104.0 ± 3.7 g). During the 3 weeks, males had a small but significant growth in length (0.18 ± 0.03 mm.day-1, t 11 = 5.74, p < 0.001) and weight (0.29 ± 0.09 g.day-1, t 11 = 3.06, p < 0.05).

Recording of the EOG and stimuli preparation

To assess the olfactory potency of urine from males of different social status, EOGs were recorded in males as described previously [28]. Briefly, male tilapia were anaesthetized in water containing 100 mg.l-1 MS222 and immobilized with gallamine triethiodide (3 mg.kg-1 in 0.9% saline). The fish were placed in a padded Perspex® V-clamp with their gills irrigated with aerated water containing MS222 (50 mg.l-1). The olfactory rosette was exposed by removing the ring of cartilage surrounding the nostril and continually irrigated with de-chlorinated, charcoal-filtered tapwater (6 ml.min-1). Stimulus-containing water was introduced into this flow via a three-way solenoid valve. At least 1 min was allowed to elapse between successive stimuli. The DC voltage was recorded by two glass micropipettes filled with 0.9% NaCl in 4% agar, one being placed close to the olfactory epithelium and the other placed lightly on the skin of the head. The signal was amplified (×103) and recorded on a PC running Axoscope software (version 1.1, Axon Instruments, Inc., Foster City, CA). The peak amplitude of the EOG response to male urine was blank-subtracted and normalized to the response to 10-5 M L-serine (standard). At the end of each recording session the fish were killed with a sharp blow to the head and measured for size and weight of body and gonads. All stimuli were prepared on the day of EOG recording using the same de-chlorinated, charcoal-filtered tapwater used to irrigate the fish's nostril. To test for a relationship between the olfactory potency of male urine (at a dilution of 1:104 in water) and the social rank of the male donor, urine taken daily from each male in the community tank was tested (60 urine stimuli) on the same male and the procedure was replicated with six males testing the samples from each donor male in random order (SL = 149.7 ± 5.6 mm; GSI = testes weight/body weight × 100 = 0.75 ± 0.07%).

Statistical analysis

The Student's t test for paired samples was used to compare the urination frequency of neighbouring males between social isolation and during the aggressive interaction. Repeated measures analysis of variance followed by the least significant difference (LSD) pairwise comparisons test were used to compare urination frequency (values transformed by log (x + 1.5)) during non-aggressive behaviours, aggressive displays and high aggression by neighbouring males. The mean urine volume collected per male, mean EOG amplitudes elicited by male urine, male size and growth were compared between five subordinate males (Di between 0 and 0.21) and seven dominant males (Di between 0.48 and 0.98) using the Student's t test for independent samples. All data are shown as means ± SEM and statistical significance was established at p < 0.05.

References

  1. Wisenden BD, Stacey NE: Fish semiochemicals and the evolution of communication networks. Animal Communication Networks. Edited by: McGregor PK. 2005, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 540-567.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  2. Stacey N, Sorensen P: Reproductive pheromones. Behaviour and Physiology of Fish. Edited by: Sloman KA, Wilson RW, Balshine S. 2006, London: Academic Press, 359-412.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Kobayashi M, Sorensen PW, Stacey NE: Hormonal and pheromonal control of spawning behavior in the goldfish. Fish Physiol Biochem. 2002, 26: 71-84. 10.1023/A:1023375931734.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Stacey N, Fraser EJ, Sorensen P, Van der Kraak G: Milt production in goldfish: regulation by multiple social stimuli. Comp Biochem Physiol C Toxicol Pharmacol. 2001, 130 (4): 467-476. 10.1016/S1532-0456(01)00273-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Fraser EJ, Stacey N: Isolation increases milt production in goldfish. J Exp Zool. 2002, 293: 511-524. 10.1002/jez.10120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Sorensen PW, Pinillos M, Scott AP: Sexually mature male goldfish release large quantities of androstenedione into the water where it functions as a pheromone. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2005, 140: 164-175. 10.1016/j.ygcen.2004.11.006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Gerlach G: Pheromonal regulation of reproductive success in female zebrafish: female suppression and male enhancement. Anim Behav. 2006, 72: 1119-1124. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Johnsson JL, Winberg S, Sloman KA: Social interactions. Behaviour and Physiology of Fish. Edited by: Sloman KA, Wilson RW, Balshine S. 2006, London: Academic Press, 151-196.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Oliveira RF: Hormones, social context and animal communication. Animal Communication Networks. Edited by: McGregor PK. 2005, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 481-520.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  10. Sloman KA, Armstrong JD: Physiological effects of dominance hierarchies: laboratory artefacts or natural phenomena?. J Fish Biol. 2002, 61: 1-23. 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2002.tb01733.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Gilmour KM, DiBattista JD, Thomas JB: Physiological causes and consequences of social status in salmonid fish. Integ Comp Biol. 2005, 45: 263-273. 10.1093/icb/45.2.263.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Loiselle PV, Barlow GW: Do fishes lek like birds?. Contrasts in behavior: adaptations in the aquatic and terrestrial environment. Edited by: Reese E, Lighter FJ. 1978, New York: John Wiley & Sons, 31-75.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Bruton MN, Boltt RE: Aspects of the biology of Tilapia mossambica Peters (Pisces: Cichlidae) in a natural freshwater lake (Lake Sibaya, South Africa). J Fish Biol. 1975, 7: 423-445. 10.1111/j.1095-8649.1975.tb04618.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Desilva SS, Sirisena HKG: Observations on the nesting habits of Oreochromis mossambicus (Peters) (Pisces, Cichlidae) in Sri-Lankan Reservoirs. J Fish Biol. 1988, 33: 689-696. 10.1111/j.1095-8649.1988.tb05514.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Fryer G, Iles TD: The Cichlid Fishes of the Great Lakes of Africa: Their Biology and Evolution. 1972, Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd

    Google Scholar 

  16. Baerends GP, Baerends van Roon JM: An introduction to the study of the ethology of cichlid fishes. Behaviour. 1950, 1 (Suppl): 1-243.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Neil EH: An analysis of colour changes and social behaviour of Tilapia mossambica. Univ Calif Publ Zool. 1964, 75: 1-58.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Oliveira RF, Almada VC: Dominance hierarchies and social structure in captive groups of the Mozambique tilapia Oreochromis mossambicus (Teleostei, Cichlidae). Ethol Ecol Evol. 1996, 8: 39-55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Oliveira RF, Almada VC, Canario AVM: Social modulation of sex steroid concentrations in the urine of male cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. Horm Behav. 1996, 30: 2-12. 10.1006/hbeh.1996.0002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Oliveira RF, Hirschenhauser K, Carneiro LA, Canario AV: Social modulation of androgen levels in male teleost fish. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol. 2002, 132 (1): 203-215. 10.1016/S1096-4959(01)00523-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Oliveira RF, Canario AVM: Hormones and social behavior of cichlid fishes: a case study in the Mozambique tilapia. J Aquar Aquat Sci. 2001, 9: 187-207.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Oliveira RF: Etologia Social e Endocrinologia Comportamental da Tilápia Oreochromis mossambicus (Teleostei, Cichlidae). PhD Thesis. 1995, Lisboa: Universidade de Lisboa

    Google Scholar 

  23. Oliveira RF, Almada VC: Dynamics of social interactions during group formation in males of the cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. Acta Ethol. 1998, 1: 57-70.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Oliveira RF, Almada VC: Mating tactics and male-male courtship in the lek-breeding cichlid Oreochromis mossambicus. J Fish Biol. 1998, 52: 1115-1129.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Turner GF: Territory dynamics and cost of reproduction in a captive population of the colonial nesting mouthbrooder Oreochromis mossambicus (Peters). J Fish Biol. 1986, 29: 573-587. 10.1111/j.1095-8649.1986.tb04974.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Almeida OG, Miranda A, Hubbard PC, Frade P, Barata EN, Canário AVM: Urine as a social signal in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus). Chem Senses. 2005, 30: i309-i310. 10.1093/chemse/bjh238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Almeida OG, Barata EN, Hubbard PC, Canário AVM: Urination rate of male tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus) is highly dependent on social context. Comp Biochem Physiol A. 2003, 134: S27-S28. 10.1016/S1095-6433(02)00145-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Frade P, Hubbard PC, Barata EN, Canário AVM: Olfactory sensitivity of the Mozambique tilapia to conspecific odours. J Fish Biol. 2002, 61: 1239-1254. 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2002.tb02468.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Miranda A, Almeida OG, Hubbard PC, Barata EN, Canário AVM: Olfactory discrimination of female reproductive status by male tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus). J Exp Biol. 2005, 208: 2037-2043. 10.1242/jeb.01584.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Amorim MCP, Fonseca PJ, Almada VC: Sound production during courtship and spawning of Oreochromis mossambicus : male-female and male-male interactions. J Fish Biol. 2003, 62: 658-667. 10.1046/j.1095-8649.2003.00054.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Poling KR, Fraser EJ, Sorensen PW: The three steroidal components of the goldfish preovulatory pheromone signal evoke different behaviors in males. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol. 2001, 129 (2-3): 645-651. 10.1016/S1096-4959(01)00361-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Cardwell JR, Stacey NE, Tan ESP, McAdam DSO, Lang SLC: Androgen increases olfactory receptor response to a vertebrate sex pheromone. J Comp Physiol A. 1995, 176: 55-61. 10.1007/BF00197752.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Cole TB, Stacey NE: Olfactory responses to steroids in an African mouth-brooding cichlid, Haplochromis burtoni (Günther). J Fish Biol. 2006, 68: 661-680. 10.1111/j.0022-1112.2006.00944.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Curtis BJ, Wood CM: The function of the urinary bladder in vivo in the freshwater rainbow trout. J Exp Biol. 1991, 155: 567-583.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Curtis BJ, Wood CM: Kidney and urinary-bladder responses of fresh-water rainbow-trout to isosmotic NaCl and NaHCO3 infusion. J Exp Biol. 1992, 173: 181-203.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Sloman KA, Scott GR, McDonald DG, Wood CM: Diminished social status affects ionoregulation at the gills and kidney in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 2004, 61: 618-626. 10.1139/f04-032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Mucignat-Caretta C, Bondi M, Caretta A: Endocrine status affects bladder size and postvoid residual urinary volume in mice. Horm Behav. 2004, 46: 11-18. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.02.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Brennan PA, Zufall F: Pheromonal communication in vertebrates. Nature. 2006, 444: 308-315. 10.1038/nature05404.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Hurst JL, Beynon RJ: Scent wars: the chemobiology of competitive signalling in mice. Bioessays. 2004, 26: 1288-1298. 10.1002/bies.20147.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Yambe H, Kitamura S, Kamio M, Yamada M, Matsunaga S, Fusetani N, Yamazaki F: L-Kynurenine, an amino acid identified as a sex pheromone in the urine of ovulated female masu salmon. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006, 103: 15370-15374. 10.1073/pnas.0604340103.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Yambe H, Yamada M, Yamazaki F: Responses of immature male masu salmon parr to the urine of mature males. Ichthyol Res. 2006, 53: 182-184. 10.1007/s10228-005-0323-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Amorim MCP, Almada VC: The outcome of male-male encounters affects subsequent sound production during courtship in the cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. Anim Behav. 2005, 69: 595-601. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hsu Y, Earley RL, Wolf LL: Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2006, 81: 33-74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Giaquinto PC, Volpato GL: Chemical communication, aggression, and conspecific recognition in the fish Nile tilapia. Physiol Behav. 1997, 62: 1333-1338. 10.1016/S0031-9384(97)00347-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Krebs JR, Davies NB: Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. 1997, Oxford: Blackwell Science, Ltd

    Google Scholar 

  46. Alcock J: Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach. 2005, Sunderland: Sinauer Associates, Inc, 8

    Google Scholar 

  47. Boehm T, Zufall F: MHC peptides and the sensory evaluation of genotype. Trends Neurosci. 2006, 29: 100-107. 10.1016/j.tins.2005.11.006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Braithwaite VA: Cognitive ability in fish. Behaviour and Physiology of Fish. Edited by: Sloman KA, Wilson RW, Balshine S. 2006, London: Academic Press, 1-37.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Grosenick L, Clement TS, Fernald RD: Fish can infer social rank by observation alone. Nature. 2007, 445: 429-432. 10.1038/nature05511.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Breithaupt T, Atema J: The timing of chemical signaling with urine in dominance fights of male lobsters (Homarus americanus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2000, 49: 67-78. 10.1007/s002650000271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Breithaupt T, Eger P: Urine makes the difference: chemical communication in fighting crayfish made visible. J Exp Biol. 2002, 205: 1221-1231.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bergman DA, Martin AL, Moore PA: Control of information flow through the influence of mechanical and chemical signals during agonistic encounters by the crayfish, Orconectes rusticus. Anim Behav. 2005, 70: 485-496. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.11.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Appelt CW, Sorensen PW: Freshwater fish release urinary pheromones in a pulsatile manner. Advances in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates. Edited by: Johnston RE, Müller-Schwarze D, Sorensen PW. 1999, New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, 247-256.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by Ministry of Science and Higher Education and European Social Funds through the Portuguese National Science Foundation (FCT), projects POCTI/BSE/38815/2001 and POCI/BIA-BDE/55463/2004. We thank João Reis (Universidade do Algarve, Faro, Portugal) for technical support. We thank the anonymous referees for their valuable comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eduardo N Barata.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

ENB devised the study, participated in the planning of all experiments, analysed the behavioural data collected with the Observer Video-Pro, performed all statistical analyses, participated in the discussion of results and wrote the manuscript. PCH participated in the conception and design of the study, planning of all experiments, supervised the recording of EOGs, contributed to discussion of results and wrote the manuscript. OGA planned and carried out the behavioural experiments with the male pairs, collected behavioural data with the Observer Video-Pro and participated in analysis and discussion of results. AM planned and carried out the behavioural observations of the males in the community tank and the recording of EOGs and contributed to statistical analysis and discussion of the results. AVMC provided resources, contributed to discussion of results and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barata, E.N., Hubbard, P.C., Almeida, O.G. et al. Male urine signals social rank in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus) . BMC Biol 5, 54 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7007-5-54

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7007-5-54

Keywords