Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

A systematic review of the development and application of home cage monitoring in laboratory mice and rats

Abstract

Background

Traditionally, in biomedical animal research, laboratory rodents are individually examined in test apparatuses outside of their home cages at selected time points. However, the outcome of such tests can be influenced by various factors and valuable information may be missed when the animals are only monitored for short periods. These issues can be overcome by longitudinally monitoring mice and rats in their home cages. To shed light on the development of home cage monitoring (HCM) and the current state-of-the-art, a systematic review was carried out on 521 publications retrieved through PubMed and Web of Science.

Results

Both the absolute (~ × 26) and relative (~ × 7) number of HCM-related publications increased from 1974 to 2020. There was a clear bias towards males and individually housed animals, but during the past decade (2011–2020), an increasing number of studies used both sexes and group housing. In most studies, animals were kept for short (up to 4 weeks) time periods in the HCM systems; intermediate time periods (4–12 weeks) increased in frequency in the years between 2011 and 2020. Before the 2000s, HCM techniques were predominantly applied for less than 12 h, while 24-h measurements have been more frequent since the 2000s. The systematic review demonstrated that manual monitoring is decreasing in relation to automatic techniques but still relevant. Until (and including) the 1990s, most techniques were applied manually but have been progressively replaced by automation since the 2000s. Independent of the year of publication, the main behavioral parameters measured were locomotor activity, feeding, and social behaviors; the main physiological parameters were heart rate and electrocardiography. External appearance-related parameters were rarely examined in the home cages. Due to technological progress and application of artificial intelligence, more refined and detailed behavioral parameters have been investigated in the home cage more recently.

Conclusions

Over the period covered in this study, techniques for HCM of mice and rats have improved considerably. This development is ongoing and further progress as well as validation of HCM systems will extend the applications to allow for continuous, longitudinal, non-invasive monitoring of an increasing range of parameters in group-housed small rodents in their home cages.

Background

In biomedical research, laboratory rodents traditionally are removed from their home cages for defined periods of time (from a few minutes up to several hours) and placed in experimental apparatuses to measure parameters of interest. The experimental apparatuses usually are unfamiliar to the animals and, therefore, represent novel environments. In several cases, experimental designs include familiarization sessions to reduce the potential impact of stress and anxiety before collecting the parameters of interest. Common examples for monitoring behavioral parameters, such as anxiety-related behavior or locomotor activity, are the Open Field and the Elevated Plus Maze tests [1]. Both tests are based on mice’s and rats’ natural avoidance of open areas (Open Field) where risk of predation is high, as well as unsafe enclosures (height with no protection in the Elevated Plus Maze), measuring exploration and locomotor activity as expressions of anxiety-like behavior [2]. However, such brief tests, performed outside of the home cage, may not adequately reflect the complexity of, for example, anxiety-like behavior [3,4,5,6,7]. Moreover, the behaviors displayed by an animal in these tests also depend on unrelated factors [8] and it has been shown that the reproducibility of test results obtained in this way is low [9].

The same applies to the collection of other than behavioral data outside the home cage. If, for instance, body temperature is measured using a rectal probe, an animal will generally be removed from its home cage and be hand-restrained for the duration of the measurements, which can result in a stress response and stress-associated hyperthermia [10].

When the animals are tested outside of their home cage, the time of day can play a critical role, too. Although rats and mice are nocturnal/crepuscular animals, experiments are often carried out during the daytime when lights are turned on [11,12,13]. In addition, it needs to be considered which behaviors are observed at what time. Mice show higher activity in an Open Field test during the dark phase when compared to the light phase [14]. Mice also consume less food during the light phase and are less motivated to work for rewards when compared to dark phase performance [15].

Other disadvantages of tests performed outside the animals’ home cage are that the animals are handled by an experimenter and separated from their familiar social group, which may negatively influence both animal welfare and data quality [16,17,18,19]. Another influencing factor is the laboratory environment. In 1999, selected strains of mice were tested under standardized conditions (same test equipment, protocols, and environmental variables) in three different laboratories. The results showed that the behavior of the mouse strains depended on the laboratory. The authors warn that experiments performed to characterize mouse strains may be laboratory dependent and therefore less strain specific [4]. To date, the reproducibility crisis remains unresolved and various approaches are discussed and pursued to improve the reliability of scientific data [4, 7, 20,21,22,23,24]. One approach that can be used to minimize variability between laboratories is to monitor the animals in their familiar environment (i.e., in their home cage). Krackow and colleagues were able to show that both the activity and spatial learning of mice tested in the home cage was constant across different laboratories [25].

Housing conditions for laboratory animals vary considerably between animal facilities around the world and there is no standard definition of a home cage. However, a home cage can generally be considered as the environment where the animals spend most of their lifetimes [26, 27]. In the European Union, this environment must meet the minimum requirements defined in the Directive 2010/63/EU. The Directive stipulates that social animals should be kept in groups whenever possible [28]. The minimum floor area of a home cage should be adjusted depending on the species, the number of animals, and their body weight (size). Food, water, bedding, and nesting material must be provided [28]. Furthermore, the home cage should be structured in a way that allows a wide range of natural behaviors to be exhibited [28]. However, a lack of space, social contact, environmental enrichment, and novelty aspects prevent the animals from displaying their full behavioral repertoire and can result in major animal welfare issues, as observed by the occurrence of stereotypic behaviors and increased aggression.

A broad range of parameters can be monitored in the home cage using manual or automated techniques, e.g., a variety of behavioral parameters such as activity [29, 30], social behavior [31,32,33], learning and memory [34,35,36], feeding [37,38,39], and physiological parameters like heart rate or body temperature [40,41,42,43]. Manual techniques used for home cage monitoring (HCM) include live observations or analyses of videos recorded by camera-based systems, which can be complemented or substituted by automated HCM techniques. Automated HCM techniques have been implemented in various commercial systems. A range of systems were described in a review by Richardson et al. [44]. For example, a combination of light-emitting diodes (LEDs), infrared light sensors, and a camera allows for analyzing, among others, physical activity or learning behavior [35, 45]. Electrical capacitance technology makes it possible to measure physical activity or rest [46, 47]. When the animals move in the cage, changes in a weak electromagnetic field can be utilized to track their movements. However, these systems do not allow the acquisition of individual data from group-housed animals. Data from individuals, also when they are kept in social groups, can be generated using radio-frequency identification (RFID) systems. For instance, the activity of 20–40 mice can be automatically measured in a large semi-naturalistic home cage [48, 49]. RFID systems enable testing learning and memory of mice and rats using operant conditioning corners that grant or deny access to water [36, 50, 51] and performing preference tests in the home cage [52, 53]. RFID systems can be combined with other techniques such as depth-sensing infrared camera for automatic individual tracking of animals [54].

HCM offers several advantages for both data quality [25, 55, 56] and animal welfare [46, 57, 58]. Ideally, group-housed animals are observed and/or tested in their familiar environment 24/7 and remain undisturbed by the experimenters. HCM has grown in importance and demands have changed over the last decades driven by technology and digitalization. However, automated 24-h measurements for long-term periods still seem to be a major challenge.

In 2021, European researchers joined forces in a project titled “Improving biomedical research by automated behaviour monitoring in the animal home-cage” (TEATIME: cost-teatime.org) in order to promote the further use and development of HCM systems. On this website, a comprehensive catalog of currently existing HCM systems is provided (cost-teatime.org/about/technologies), which will be further updated. Approximately at the same time, another initiative called “Translational Digital Biomarkers” was launched within the North American 3Rs Collaborative (www.na3rsc.org/tdb). To identify past trends in and the current state of HCM, a systematic review was carried out in close collaboration with the COST Action TEATIME network. The following research questions were addressed: How have techniques and applications for home cage monitoring of laboratory mice and rats developed over time? Has the degree of automatization for monitoring behavioral, physiological, and external appearance-related parameters changed?

Results

Overview of the studies

The search through PubMed and Web of Science retrieved 1079 and 341 references, respectively. Figure 1 shows the PRISMA flow diagram. A total of 241 duplicates was removed. Titles and abstracts of 1179 references were screened, of which the full text for 721 references was assessed for eligibility. Five hundred twenty-one references were retained for data extraction.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram

Increasing number of publications on HCM

The earliest publication fulfilling our criteria for home cage monitoring (Additional file 1) that we could find was published in 1974. In 2018, 2019 and 2020, 29, 27 and 26 publications were identified, respectively. The yearly number of publications in which mice and/or rats were monitored in their home cages according to our eligibility criteria increased (Fig. 2A). In 1987, no publication meeting the inclusion criteria was found. In 2021, only one study fulfilled the eligibility criteria, but since the literature search was carried out in February 2021 the databases did not cover the entire year.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Historical change in the publication rate. A Absolute number of included publications on home cage monitoring (HCM) of mice and rats (n = 520 publications). B The ratio of HCM references (n = 520 publications) relative to the overall number of studies in mice or rats (n = 3,124,961 publications) published in the years 1974 to 2020. The overall number of studies in mice or rats was retrieved through a search in PubMed using the search string: “mice”[MeSH Terms] OR “mice”[All Fields] OR “rats”[MeSH Terms] OR “rats”[All Fields]; filter: other animals)

When comparing the number of HCM publications with the overall number of studies in mice or rats listed in PubMed (search string: “mice”[MeSH Terms] OR “mice”[All Fields] OR “rats”[MeSH Terms] OR “rats”[All Fields]; filter: other animals), the percentage of relevant HCM references increased over time (Fig. 2B; linear regression analysis: F(1, 45) = 100.704, p < 0.001; R2 = 0.691).

High number of studies using male mice or rats only

Mice were studied in 276 publications, rats in 240, and both species in 5 publications (Table 1). Over all studies, 61% used male animals only. The percentage of studies exclusively using males was 55% in mice (13% females only), 69% in rats (11% females only), and 40% in studies of both species (20% females only). Both sexes were used in 28% of mouse studies, 19% of rat studies, and 20% in studies of both species. For the past 10 years (2011–2020), the percentage of included studies using animals of both sexes increased (Fig. 3A).

Table 1 Species and sex of animals used in the included publications
Fig. 3
figure 3

Historical change in the number of publications using male and/or female mice (A), publications investigating specific disease models (B), and publications employing single or group housing (C) (n = 520 publications)

Frequency of strains used

The most frequently used mouse strains were C57BL/6 (n = 192), BALB/c (n = 31), CD-1 (n = 26), DBA/2 (n = 21), and 129 (n = 16), whereas the most frequently used rat stocks were Sprague Dawley (n = 115), Wistar (n = 74), and Long Evans (n = 34) (Fig. 4). In mice inbred strains dominate, whereas in rats outbred strains prevail. In Additional file 2, the numbers of publications including particular strains, even those subsumed under “other” in Fig. 4, are listed.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mouse and rat strains that were used in the included publications in the years 1974 to 2021 (data obtained from n = 521 publications). More than one strain could be used in a study. Strains that were indicated twice or once only, were summarized under “other”

HCM studies involving disease models increased within the past two decades

The disease models were categorized according to ICD-11 (https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en). The most frequently studied disease models in the included references were mental, behavioral, or neurodevelopmental disorders (n = 74), diseases of the nervous system (n = 32), and endocrine, nutritional or metabolic diseases (n = 15; Table 2). In a majority of publications—368 references—no disease model was studied. However, the percentage of included studies involving disease models increased in the two past decades (Fig. 3B; 1974–1980: 20%; 1981–1990: 13%; 1991–2000: 18%; 2001–2010: 27%; 2011–2020: 38%).

Table 2 Disease models used in the included publications

Growing use of commercially available HCM systems

Additional file 3 provides detailed information about the HCM systems used in the included studies. The focus of this table are the techniques and systems specifically developed for the purpose of home cage monitoring. The table contains the number of publications in which the HCM systems were used, and the species, strain/stock, sex, and group size of the animals housed in the systems. Moreover, the duration the animals spent in the particular system is listed. In 126 cases custom-built HCM systems and in 219 cases commercially available HCM systems were used (multiple HCM systems were applied in 28 studies). A “custom-built” system had to fulfill two criteria: (1) it is not commercially available and construction plans and/or software were provided by the authors; (2) the authors gave a name to the system. If these criteria were not met and neither a commercially available nor a “custom-built” system was used, it was considered that no system was applied in the study, e.g., the deployment of a camera only. In 209 publications, no HCM system was used/indicated; in 16 of these 209 publications, a HCM system was used without indicating its name. The use of commercially available HCM systems has increased over time (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Historical change in the number of custom-built and commercially available home cage monitoring (HCM) systems. More than one system could be applied in a study. In n = 126 cases custom-built HCM systems and in n = 219 cases commercially available HCM systems were used (multiple HCM systems were applied in n = 28 studies). Studies in which no HCM system was used were excluded from this figure

Single housing remained the most common housing condition

In most studies (57%), animals were individually housed in their home cage system (n = 298 publications) (Table 3). In the 1970s, there were as many studies in which animals were kept individually as in groups (Fig. 3C; 1974–1980: 47% group housing, 47% single housing). After 1980, group housing has decreased in comparison to the 1970s, but there was a slight increasing trend between the 1990s and the 2010s (Fig. 3C; 1974–1980: 47%, 1981–1990: 32%, 1991–2000: 31%, 2001–2010: 33%, 2011–2020: 38%). In contrast, the number of studies with single-housed animals has increased after 1980 in comparison to the 1970s and slightly decreased again in the 2000s and 2010s (1974–1980: 47%, 1981–1990: 61%, 1991–2000: 61%, 2001–2010: 59%, 2011–2020: 55%). Table 3 shows that those laboratory rodents that were socially housed were often kept in pairs (n = 60 publications) more so than in groups of three (n = 27 publications) or four animals (n = 30 publications). Other group sizes between five and nine animals were even less frequently found. Groups of ten (n = 12 publications) or even more than ten animals (n = 16 publications) were rarely reported in the included publications. It must be noted that the indicated numbers were the maximum numbers of animals housed in a cage.

Table 3 Maximum number of animals housed together in a cage

Additional file 4 suggests that in single-sex studies individual housing may be more common for male mice than female mice, though this data must be interpreted with caution. For rats, both sexes were predominantly single-housed.

Animals were mostly kept in the HCM systems for short time periods

The duration of housing was classified as short-term (1–28 days), intermediate (1–3 months), or long-term (more than 3 months). Regardless of the publication year, most studies used short-term housing (2–7 days: n = 75; 1–2 weeks: n = 73; 2–4 weeks: n = 102). Intermediate duration housing was not uncommon (4–12 weeks; n = 127; Fig. 6A). However, single-day housing for HCM (n = 11) and longer periods (12–24 weeks, n = 35; 24–48 weeks, n = 8; more than 48 weeks, n = 7) were rare. In 16% of the studies (n = 83), the authors did not indicate the duration of housing.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Historical change in the duration of housing and monitoring in the home cage: A duration spent in the home cage (n = 520 publications), B overall duration of measurement (n = 667 techniques), C duration of measurement per day (n = 699 techniques) using a home cage monitoring technique. For B and C, it must be noted that more than one technique for data measurement could be used in a study. If one of the techniques used to investigate parameters of interests could not be extracted from the publication, this technique was excluded for B and C. Note the differences in the scaling of the y-axis

Increasing number of studies conducting measurements during the entire time the animals spent in a HCM system

Figure 6B gives information on how long a technique was used for measuring behavioral, physiological, or external appearance–related parameters. In all years, most studies involved measurements of 2–7 days. In 1974–1980, the animals were mostly monitored for 1–14 days. Only in rare cases did the measurements last for 4–24 weeks. In 1981–1990, the proportion of studies in which parameters were analyzed for 2–12 weeks increased. Between 2011 and 2020, the frequencies of measurements over 1 day, 1–2 weeks, 2–4 weeks, and 4–12 weeks were relatively similar, with the latter being clearly increased in comparison to the two past decades. Longer periods of measurement (i.e., > 3 months) were only found in a few cases.

There was a slight change over time in the relation between the housing duration in the HCM systems and the duration for which the parameters of interest were measured. It has become more common to do measurements for the entire period in which the animals are kept in a HCM system over time (1974–1980: 29%; 1981–1990: 44%; 1991–2000: 42%; 2001–2010: 47%; 2011–2020: 54%).

Duration of monitoring per day in a HCM system increased

As shown in Fig. 6C, in the time period 1974–2000, most measurements lasted for less than 12 h per day. Past the year 2000, 24-h measurements were conducted more frequently.

Overall, automated techniques dominated for measurements of more than 12 h. For monitoring periods of less than 12 h, manual techniques were common (70% of techniques).

A large range of manual and automatic HCM techniques can be applied for collecting data from group-housed animals

In most studies, one HCM technique was used (n = 362). The second most frequently reported option was the combination of two techniques (n = 118). A combination of three (n = 35) or more (n = 6) techniques was less often applied.

Table 4 shows that in a group housing setting, more techniques were applied to measure data from individuals than from groups of animals. Approximately as many manual as automatic HCM techniques were used to generate data from individual subjects. Data from groups of animals were rather collected by manual techniques.

Table 4 Percentage of automatic and manual techniques measuring data from individuals and groups of animals in a group-housed setting

The techniques listed in Table 5 were used in group-housed animals and almost all of them allowed for generating data for individual animals. However, it must be considered that often two or more techniques were combined, which may allow for collecting data from individuals instead of animal groups only (e.g., by combining a RFID system, which identifies individual animals, with other techniques).

Table 5 Percentage of techniques that were applied for measuring data from individuals or groups of animals in a group-housed setting

Figure 7A illustrates how the use of techniques for home cage monitoring of laboratory mice and rats has developed over time. Across the whole study periods, manual evaluation (including live monitoring and manual evaluation of videos from RGB or infrared cameras) was the most frequently used technique. In the 2010s, non-invasive methods (i.e., beam-based tracking and visual object tracking) have overtaken the use of telemetry, which is extremely invasive. It should be noted that telemetry and the non-invasive methods mentioned can be automated.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Historical change in home cage monitoring techniques for laboratory mice and rats (data obtained from n = 520 publications). More than one technique could be indicated in a publication. If one of the techniques used to investigate parameters of interests could not be extracted from the publication but information on the degree of automatization was given, this technique was excluded for A and included for B. A Number of techniques used in the included publications in the defined time periods. Other: infrared thermometer (n = 1), automatic food dispenser (n = 1), impedance pneumography (n = 1), flowmeter circuit (n = 1), lever (n = 2), brain imaging cameras (n = 2), fiber photometry system (n = 1), cardiotachometer (n = 1), thermal imaging (n = 1). B Number of manual and automated techniques that were applied in the publications included

In the periods between 1974–1980, 1981–1990, and 1991–2000, most measurements were performed manually (71%; 57%; 59%) (Fig. 7B). In contrast, between 2001–2010 and 2011–2020 (52% and 68%), most measurements were automated.

In Tables 6, 7, and 8, the techniques are separated into methods for assessing behavior, physiology, and external appearance. Manual evaluation and visual object tracking (video) played a role for almost all behavioral parameters (Table 6). Among other techniques, photo beam (or photo diode)-based tracking and RFID were used to analyze locomotor activity, wheel running, motor and sensory functions, feeding, social behavior, and learning and memory. RFID systems were used for studying spatial preference, and abnormal behavior. Less frequently used techniques can be found in Table 6.

Table 6 Techniques used for the analysis of behavioral parameters
Table 7 Techniques used for the analysis of physiological parameters
Table 8 Techniques used for the analysis of external appearance-related parameters

Telemetry has been used to record most physiological parameters, except for body weight and hormone levels (Table 7). It must be noted that hormone concentrations were measured using microdialysis, automated blood sampling, or in fecal samples. The latter were manually collected from the home cages and analyzed later. In the context of the present systematic review, the collection of fecal samples for subsequent analysis was considered a form of manual evaluation. For the analysis of neuronal activity, telemetry was used in most cases. Respiration was mainly manually assessed. Other techniques such as RFID, electronic sensors and transducers, vibration, force, and weight-sensitive tracking, as well as tissue sampling (e.g., microdialysis) were used less frequently.

External appearance was usually evaluated manually (Table 8).

Detailed information on the methodology of each behavioral, physiological, and external appearance-related parameter (i.e., the degree of automatization, group versus individual monitoring, and the duration of measurement) can be found in Additional file 5.

Behavioral parameters most frequently investigated in the home cage were locomotor activity, feeding, and social behavior

Figure 8 illustrates the distribution of behavioral, physiological, and external appearance-related parameters between 1974 and 2020. Categories of behavioral parameters (Fig. 8A; a range of behaviors were subsumed under a category) that have been investigated throughout were locomotor activity, feeding, social behavior, abnormal behavior, facial expressions and body posture, as well as learning and memory—with locomotor activity, feeding, and social behavior being the most frequently examined parameters. Locomotor activity stands out in particular. While defecation and urination were already examined in the 1970s, wheel running, grooming, vocalization, sleep behavior, and spatial preference were studied in the home cage for the first time in the 1980s. Observations of burrowing and nesting, or other home cage behaviors used in the modelling of human disorders such as anxiety, depression, and schizophrenia were first reported in the 1990s. Sniffing as well as motor and sensory functions became of interest in the 2000s. Seizures were monitored in the home cage for the first time in the 2010s.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Historical change in monitoring of A behavioral parameters, B physiological parameters, and C external appearance-related parameters in mice and rats in their home cages (data obtained from n = 520 publications). More than one application (i.e., parameter) could be indicated in a publication. If the same parameter was investigated using different techniques in a study, the parameter was counted only once for this study. Examples for abnormal behaviors were infanticide, barbering, and stereotypy. Other behaviors (each n = 1): sneezing, sign of pain or distress (not further specified), clinical signs (not further specified), curiosity/alertness, champing behavior, twitches, and behavior not further specified. Note the differences in the scaling of the y-axis

Physiological parameters most frequently investigated in the home cage were heart rate and/or electrocardiography

Among the physiological parameters (Fig. 8B), heart rate and/or electrocardiography were the most popular and were reported in all periods. The analysis of body temperature in the home cage became relevant in the 1980s. Its use has increased over time. It was the second most frequently examined physiological parameter in the 2000s and 2010s. Blood pressure and respiration have been measured in the home cage since 1983, neuronal activity (including electroencephalograms from implanted electrodes) since 1998, and body weight since 2017. The analysis of electromyography and hormone concentrations in the home cage was first reported between 2001 and 2010; but even later, they were investigated in a few studies only. In 2001–2010, blood pressure was the third most popular physiological parameter; in 2011–2020, neuronal activity was examined more often than blood pressure.

External appearance-related parameters were rarely investigated in the home cage

External appearance-related parameters have been investigated since 1988 but very few studies focused on these aspects as readouts (Fig. 8C).

Discussion

In the present systematic review, based on 521 references retrieved through PubMed and Web of Science (until Feb 2021), we studied yearly changes in HCM of laboratory mice and rats between 1974 and 2020. We found an increase in the use of HCM over time. The number of studies using animals of both sexes has grown, as has the study of specific disease models since the last decade(s). Over time, novel HCM techniques have been introduced and the degree of automatization has increased, allowing monitoring of more challenging animal-based parameters in the home cage. Moreover, longer housing in HCM systems with continuous monitoring, also under social housing conditions, are some of the key developments we were able to note with respect to the monitoring of mice and rats in their home cages.

From heterogeneity to harmonization of the home cage definition

The home cage definition played a central role for the present systematic review since references were only included if the following criteria were met: a home cage must allow animals to be housed permanently in their familiar social structure. We considered it important that the structure should not be changed for monitoring the parameters of interest, since this could influence the data and compromise animal welfare [16, 19]. However, animals that were kept individually throughout the process of monitoring also met our home cage definition. This definition excluded all studies in which group-housed animals were separated for the purpose of testing.

The definition of a home cage varies in the literature and may change over time with new scientific findings and ethical norms. For instance, the definition by Baran et al. focused on the time the animals spent in the “cages […] where animals are housed for the majority of their lifetime in the vivarium”, i.e., their home cage [27]. In contrast, the definition of the present systematic review concentrated on the social structure.

To harmonize the definition of a home cage, experts in the field of HCM recently made efforts to develop a more detailed definition: www.cost-teatime.org/about/hcm-definition. This definition is structured as an olog (i.e., a categorical framework for knowledge representation) [59]. It can be used for checking whether a system fulfills the criteria of HCM and it may serve as a future reporting guideline for HCM systems. However, it must be noted that the HCM definition for this systematic review was developed before the ongoing discussion on terminology and could not be updated after registration of the systematic review protocol.

Limitations due to search term, exclusion criteria, and date of search

Since the search term used in the present systematic review contained the term “home cage” but no alternative spellings or descriptions except from “housing cage”, some HCM articles may have been missed. For example, publications that did not explicitly focus on HCM and therefore did not use this term, may have not been identified by our search strategy. Moreover, HCM may no longer be mentioned in articles when it has already become a routine procedure in a laboratory and considered as standard. Additionally, the terminology of HCM can depend on the research field. For example, researchers studying natural behavior may use the term “home cage monitoring,” while other researchers assessing learning in the home cage may call it “automated group-housed learning”.

However, in the absence of a harmonized definition of “home cage,” the lack of consensus on the use of descriptors and key words, and the widespread use of HCM in a range of research fields, a comprehensive search of HCM publications would have been nearly impossible.

Besides the home cage definition and terminology, the inclusion and exclusion criteria limited the number of studies that were considered. Studies involving only calorimetric measurements in the home cage were excluded since the animals usually must be transferred to a metabolic cage where they are individually housed for a short term (i.e., they are separated from their social group). Due to this exclusion criteria, only those metabolic studies were included in which also other parameters (e.g., locomotor activity) than metabolic outcomes were investigated.

Publication rate

The absolute number of publications in which mice and/or rats were monitored in their home cages appeared to increase from 1974 to 2020, especially around 2005 and in the following years. Around 2005, instrumental “home cage-like” test cages were brought to the market [6, 27, 60]. A few years later, home cages with capacitance sensors, RFID, and/or video tracking were developed [27]. The results of our systematic review demonstrated that the use of commercial systems has increased considerably. Custom-built systems were frequently used until the 1990s. Since the 2000s, it appeared that more commercial than custom-built systems were used. According to our definition of custom-built systems, in 209 studies no system was used or indicated. In 16 of these 209 publications, the name of a HCM system that seemed to have been used was not indicated which does not allow to conclude whether a commercial or custom-built system was applied.

The high proportion of commercially available HCM systems used in recent years suggested that researchers increasingly monitored animals in their home cages since HCM systems were available on the market and more easily accessible. However, custom-built set-ups remained important tools, indicating that the HCM field is still in its early development and commercial systems currently cannot fully satisfy the demand of the user community. Another reason for the popularity of custom-built systems may be that open-source custom-built systems are more transparent and allow for faster development and adoption of the techniques. This is in line with the move towards open science and improving reproducibility. In future work, it may be of interest to determine whether commercial and custom-built HCM systems are used to investigate parameters of different complexity. It may be hypothesized that “simple” parameters are investigated using commercial HCM systems, but custom-built systems are required for more complex parameters. However, the HCM systems were not assigned to the parameters investigated in the present systematic review, and therefore the results do not provide this information.

In relation to the total number of PubMed-listed studies including mice or rats, the percentage of HCM publications increased over time. The overall annual publication growth rate in Life Science is 5% according to Bornemann et al. [61].

Sex bias

We found a strong male-bias for both mice and rats, as reported previously for non-human mammals in other fields of biomedical research [62]. Beery and Zucker evaluated journal articles from 2009 and found a male skew in “neuroscience, physiology, pharmacology, endocrinology, and zoology” [62]. Flórez-Vargas et al. revealed a strong male skew in some mouse models, such as cardiovascular disease models, between 2001 and 2014 [63]. Male animals were often preferred by researchers intending to reduce potential higher data variability related the different phases of the estrous cycle in females [62]. However, the evidence for this is contentious [64,65,66,67,68]. A female bias was found in reproduction and immunology [62], and infectious disease mouse models [63]. Approximately 15% of studies published in the Journal of Physiology (London) and the Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics between 1909 and 2009 used non-human mammals of both sexes [62]. This percentage was a little higher (24% over all years) in the studies included in our systematic review and, interestingly, increased since the 2010s (29%)—probably due to a growing awareness of the consequences of a sex bias in animal-based research [69]. Finally, we would like to note the considerable advantages of testing both male and female rodents in basic research studies to better explore diseases which display variable prevalence in males and females.

HCM allows social housing

Under natural conditions, the house mouse (Mus musculus) lives in relative stable social groups of up to 10 mice/m2. The social structure is a despotic dominance hierarchy with a dominant male, several subordinates, and several females and their offspring. Rank fights may occur between males and may result in the death of a male. A dominance hierarchy is also established among females, in which only the dominant females reproduce. Rats, on the other hand, live in larger colonies with several hundred individuals of both sexes. They live in polygenic societies with promiscuous mating and low agonistic behavior (reviewed in [70]). The conditions under which laboratory animals are kept vary widely.

Our systematic review revealed that male mice and rats of both sexes were individually housed in the majority of studies, although individual housing can impair the emotional state of the animals. In mice, social isolation is known to increase anxiety-related behavior and depressive states [71, 72], to elevate corticosterone concentrations and reduce levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [73]. Moreover, social deprivation was shown to affect how mice react to a stressor [74]. Social deprivation is associated with welfare concerns and can result in changes in behavioral, physiological, and neurochemical parameters, metabolism, brain structures, and processes [75,76,77]. In neurodegenerative mouse models, single housing can affect the disease progression [78]. According to our systematic review, the number of studies in which laboratory rodents were individually housed has slightly decreased in the 2000s and 2010s when compared to the previous two decades. This may also be due to the technological progress in the last decades and availability of HCM systems allowing group housing. However, because of inter-individual aggression with the onset of sexual maturity, male mice are often separated from their same-sex cage mates to avoid stress, and injuries [79,80,81]. It is worth noting that aggressive behavior in group housed male mice seems also to be affected by the group and cage size [82, 83]. In the studies included in our systematic review, female mice, in contrast to male mice, were more likely to be kept in groups. This may be explained by the feasibility of group housing for females.

The ability to obtain individual data from group-housed animals allows the housing conditions to be adapted to the needs of the animals. Furthermore, depending on the system and group size, several animals could be tested at the same time, which could counteract a possible batch effect. However, the development of the social structure under laboratory conditions, including available space and environmental enrichment in the HCM system, must be taken into account when interpreting the data.

Duration of housing and monitoring in the home cage

In most studies included in our review, the laboratory rodents were kept for periods of 2 days to 3 months in the HCM systems. It should be noted that data generated from animals that were only kept in the system for a few days before measurement may be biased by the novel environment and/or handling. If animals are transferred from another cage to a HCM system, it can take them a while to adapt to this novel environment. The transfer to a HCM system is comparable to a cage change. The olfactory cues as well as the familiar visual and tactile environments are removed and replaced by a clean cage, bedding, nest material, and enrichment items, which is potentially stressful for animals that strongly rely on scent for communication [84]. This can influence behavioral and physiological parameters [27, 46, 85], such as sleep [86], activity patterns [46], breathing rate [85, 87], heart rate and mean arterial blood pressure [88, 89], and corticosterone concentrations [90]. Since the animals usually are handled when cages are changed, the effects listed above may not only be due to a novel environment but can also be associated with the handling techniques used [18, 91].

Since habituation times are rarely reported, we refrained from extracting the time the animals were allowed to acclimatize to the respective HCM. Instead, data on the duration of measurement using the HCM techniques were collected. HCM techniques were mainly applied for short-term periods (i.e., 1–28 days). The number of techniques being applied for intermediate time periods (i.e., 4–12 weeks) slightly increased over time in the years between 2011 and 2020; however, 2–7 days remained the most frequent duration of measurement. Only in rare cases, HCM techniques were applied for long-term periods (i.e., more than 3 months), meaning only few longitudinal studies were carried out. However, the number of studies involving measurements during the entire time in which the animals were kept in the HCM systems slightly increased over the years, which may relate back to the development of data capture, storage, and analysis techniques.

Towards 24/7 automated home cage monitoring

HCM techniques were increasingly used for continuous data recording. Our systematic review provides evidence that the development towards 24/7 surveillance has gone hand in hand with automatization. Since the 2000s, a majority of applied HCM techniques have collected data automatically, and measurements have been recorded for 24 h a day. Long-term automated 24/7 surveillance of the animals in their home cage will allow for unbiased measurements.

An important observation is the inter-individual variation in spite of genetic homogeneity and strict standardization of husbandry and experimental conditions. This inter-individual variation can also be observed in HCM studies. It has been shown that differences in activity are more stable over time, which allows more predictability [49, 92,93,94]. In HCM studies, individuals can be observed over a longer period of time, which can lead to a better understanding of the increased susceptibility or decreased resilience of subpopulations. This is a major advance for phenotyping, monitoring of effect size of interventions, and animal welfare.

Twenty-four hours HCM takes into account the normal circadian rhythm of behavioral and physiological parameters and will reveal any phase shift relative to the light–dark-cycle (for review, see [95]). When nocturnal animals such as mice or rats are monitored for a few hours only, and then often during the light phase, valuable information may be missed, as Eikelboom and Lattanzio showed for running wheel activity [15, 96].

Automatization allows for monitoring of animals without the presence of an experimenter. Prey animals such as mice and rats may show altered behavior and hide for instance pain, suffering, or distress [97]. Therefore, live cage-side health checks may not detect impaired well-being. Automated systems can thus be an important supplement to professional visual inspection of animal health and well-being. However, in our view, the cage-side checks performed by the animal care staff should never fully be replaced since there is always the risk of technical failures. It is widely discussed that an experimenter can influence animal-based parameters [98]. For example, the odor of male experimenters was shown to cause stress and stress-induced analgesia in mice and rats [99]. However, a recent multi-laboratory study demonstrated that experimenters caused less data variation than the different laboratories [100].

In contrast to manual evaluation, automatization allows for more unbiased and data-driven assessment of parameters, which can save labor time once an automated method is established.

Use of novel techniques allows for monitoring sophisticated parameters

Between 1974 and 1980, besides the manual evaluation of videos or live observations, beam-based tracking [101, 102], telemetry [40], and electronic sensors and transducers [103, 104] were the first techniques applied for HCM. Interestingly, manual evaluation is still relevant in recent times and was the most frequently used technique in all time periods. The role of beam-based tracking, telemetry, and RFID systems became more and more important over time. Moreover, visual object tracking (video) has experienced a major boost (i.e., an increase in use) since the 2010s. As data processing technology has advanced, an increased number of data parameters can be collected in the home cage.

In the 2010s, there was a shift in technology with non-invasive methods (i.e., beam-based tracking and visual object tracking) overtaking invasive methods (i.e., telemetry), which may indicate a shift towards improved animal welfare. The use of implanted telemetry devices compromises animal welfare. Thus, in the interest of animal welfare, non-invasive techniques should be preferred over those that are associated with a burden on the animals [105]. Although not all parameters measured by telemetry can be analyzed using beam-based tracking and visual object tracking, activity can be monitored using non-invasive methods. Moreover, non-invasive alternatives such as jacketed telemetry could be applied for assessing respiration, collecting an electrocardiogram, or measuring activity [106].

Most studies conducted so far with HCM (69%) used only one technique. However, studies collecting a larger set of parameters or extracting data from individuals among group-housed animals usually deployed multiple techniques to increase the versatility of the HCM system. The present systematic review revealed an increase in the number of HCM studies involving disease models, which may be attributed to the technical progress to examine more and more parameters in the home cage and the demand to characterize a rapidly growing number of genetic mutants since the 2000s.

External appearance-related parameters (e.g., body condition score, wounds, fur condition, piloerection, chromodacryorrhea) have only rarely been quantified in the home cage yet although they are crucial for tracking the animals’ welfare status. We envision that machine learning approaches will play a central role in HCM data processing and in the future may also enable automatic monitoring of external appearance-related parameters. Unsupervised and supervised machine learning can be used to automatically analyze video frames recorded in the home cage [107,108,109].

Conclusions

All in all, our systematic review revealed that HCM in mice and rats has gone through a considerable development and, as these instruments have become more comprehensive, easy to apply, and scalable, the use of HCM has increased. There is a slight trend towards recordings covering 24 h over intermediate time periods as the HCM techniques are refined and become more and more automated, and application of HCM is spreading to a wider range of study types. A considerable fraction of the HCM systems is still custom-built, but commercial “key ready” solutions are increasingly available and since the 2000s more than 50% of the HCM studies used commercial products.

Although manual observations made live or from videos remain key technical solutions for HCM, this review indicates that a number of alternative, high-throughput, and man-power saving techniques have been introduced.

Future inventions will pave the way for continuous non-invasive rodent monitoring in longitudinal studies. Storage and analysis of the large amounts of data generated by automated HCM are a bottleneck that needs to be addressed [27]. Moreover, the financial aspects may be a hurdle in implementing commercial systems. Nevertheless, the development of new recording and analysis methods is rapid and, moreover, many of these are freely available. We, therefore, see a paradigm shift towards more and more home-cage based methods for the coming research.

Methods

Review protocol

The protocol of the systematic review was uploaded to the Open Science Framework on May 3, 2021, after the search for literature was conducted: https://osf.io/4gzcx. It was registered on March 14, 2022 when the full text review and extraction phase were in progress: https://osf.io/un5e6.

Home cage definition

A home cage was defined as any cage in which the animals could potentially be housed permanently in their familiar social structure (i.e., group or single housing). A testing apparatus could be connected to the home cage allowing the animals to voluntarily enter it. In this scenario, the animals may also separate themselves from the group when entering the testing apparatus. The home cage could be permanently or temporarily divided so that the animals are separated from each other by a cage divider (e.g., a grid). Our home cage definition excluded any cage system that required the separation of group-housed mice or rats for the duration of monitoring. In contrast, if an animal was permanently kept socially isolated in a cage system, the home cage definition was met.

Search strategy and screening

Primary databases were searched through PubMed and Web of Science on February 24, 2021. PubMed search terms were refined using the search refiner tool QueryVis [110]. The following search strings were used for obtaining relevant studies.

PubMed: ((“mice”[MeSH Terms] OR mice OR mouse) OR (“rats”[MeSH Terms] OR rat OR rats)) AND ((home cage) OR (housing cage)) AND (monitoring OR (“observation”[MeSH Terms] OR observation)). The filter “other animals” was applied.

Web of Science: ALL FIELDS: (((mice OR mouse) OR (rats OR rat)) AND ((“home cage”) OR (“housing cage”)) AND (monitoring OR observation)).

The data extraction template can be found in Additional file 6. Further detailed information on the objective and materials and methods is provided in Additional file 1. In brief, only primary studies (published in English language) in which behavioral, physiological, and/or external appearance-related parameters were monitored in mice and/or rats in their familiar social structure in their home cage or a testing apparatus connected to the home cage were included. The definition of a home cage can be found in Additional file 1. Studies in which no other parameters than calorimetric measurements were examined in a home cage were excluded. In phase 1, titles and abstracts were screened. Thereafter, full texts were screened (phase 2) and data were extracted (phase 3) simultaneously. In all phases, all papers were screened by two independent reviewers and discrepancies were resolved by a third reviewer.

Analysis

The responses for the mouse/rat strain were reviewed by two persons: In mice, the 129 substrains were subsumed under “129,” C57BL/6 J and C57BL/6N under “C57BL/6,” and ICR and CD-1 under “CD-1.” In rats, Holtzman and Sprague Dawley were subsumed under “Sprague Dawley.” For genetically modified animals, the background strain was extracted. If strains were of a mixed genetic background, “mixed background” was indicated.

If responses appeared not to be plausible or free text-responses had to be clustered, the answers were corrected and the corrected responses were used for further analysis. All original and corrected responses can be found in Additional file 7.

SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) was used for the linear regression analysis and for creating the figures. The year 2021 was excluded in the figures visualizing the historical change since the literature search was carried out in February 2021 and did not cover the entire year. The ratio of relevant HCM references per year was analyzed using a linear regression model. The R2 (fraction of explained variance) indicates how well the model fits the data (from 1, best prediction to 0, no prediction). The remaining data were descriptively analyzed.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files. The dataset can be found in Additional file 7.

Abbreviations

TEATIME COST Action:

“Improving biomedical research by automated behaviour monitoring in the animal home-cage”

HCM:

Home cage monitoring

LED:

Light-emitting diodes

RFID:

Radio-frequency identification

References

  1. Crawley JN. What’s wrong with my mouse?: behavioral phenotyping of transgenic and knockout mice. 2nd ed. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2007.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Crawley JN. Exploratory behavior models of anxiety in mice. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1985;9:37–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Chesler EJ, Wilson SG, Lariviere WR, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Mogil JS. Influences of laboratory environment on behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:1101–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Crabbe JC, Wahlsten D, Dudek BC. Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environment. Science. 1999;284:1670–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Schuessler BP, Zambetti PR, Kukuoka KM, Kim EJ, Kim JJ. The risky closed economy: a holistic, longitudinal approach to studying fear and anxiety in rodents. Front Behav Neurosci. 2020;14: 594568.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Spruijt BM, DeVisser L. Advanced behavioural screening: automated home cage ethology. Drug Discov Today Technol. 2006;3:231–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Voikar V, Gaburro S. Three pillars of automated home-cage phenotyping of mice: novel findings, refinement, and reproducibility based on literature and experience. Front Behav Neurosci. 2020;14: 575434.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Kas MJ, Van Ree JM. Dissecting complex behaviours in the post-genomic era. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:366–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Rudeck J, Vogl S, Banneke S, Schönfelder G, Lewejohann L. Repeatability analysis improves the reliability of behavioral data. PLoS ONE. 2020;15: e0230900.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Clement JG, Mills P, Brockway B. Use of telemetry to record body temperature and activity in mice. J Pharmacol Methods. 1989;21:129–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Gomes MGS, Tractenberg SG, Orso R, Viola TW, Grassi-Oliveira R. Sex differences in risk behavior parameters in adolescent mice: Relationship with brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Lett. 2022;766: 136339.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Habedank A, Kahnau P, Lewejohann L. Alternate without alternative: neither preference nor learning explains behaviour of C57BL/6J mice in the T-maze. Behaviour. 2021;158:625–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Matto V, Harro J, Allikmets L. The effects of cholecystokinin A and B receptor antagonists on exploratory behaviour in the elevated zero-maze in rat. Neuropharmacology. 1997;36:389–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Valentinuzzi VS, Buxton OM, Chang AM, Scarbrough K, Ferrari EA, Takahashi JS, et al. Locomotor response to an open field during C57BL/6J active and inactive phases: differences dependent on conditions of illumination. Physiol Behav. 2000;69:269–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Acosta J, Bussi IL, Esquivel M, Höcht C, Golombek DA, Agostino PV. Circadian modulation of motivation in mice. Behav Brain Res. 2020;382: 112471.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Manouze H, Ghestem A, Poillerat V, Bennis M, Ba-M’hamed S, Benoliel JJ, et al. Effects of single cage housing on stress, cognitive, and seizure parameters in the rat and mouse pilocarpine models of epilepsy. eNeuro. 2019;6:ENEURO.0179-18.2019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Gouveia K, Hurst JL. Reducing mouse anxiety during handling: effect of experience with handling tunnels. PLoS ONE. 2013;8: e66401.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Hurst JL, West RS. Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat Methods. 2010;7:825–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Krohn T, Sørensen D, Ottesen J, Hansen AK. The effects of individual housing on mice and rats: a review. Anim Welf. 2006;15:343–52.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Prinz F, Schlange T, Asadullah K. Believe it or not: how much can we rely on published data on potential drug targets? Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2011;10:712.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Macleod M, Mohan S. Reproducibility and rigor in animal-based research. ILAR J. 2019;60:17–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Richter SH, von Kortzfleisch V. It is time for an empirically informed paradigm shift in animal research. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2020;21:660.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. von Kortzfleisch VT, Karp NA, Palme R, Kaiser S, Sachser N, Richter SH. Improving reproducibility in animal research by splitting the study population into several “mini-experiments.” Sci Rep. 2020;10:16579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Diederich K, Schmitt K, Schwedhelm P, Bert B, Heinl C. A guide to open science practices for animal research. PLoS Biol. 2022;20: e3001810.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Krackow S, Vannoni E, Codita A, Mohammed AH, Cirulli F, Branchi I, et al. Consistent behavioral phenotype differences between inbred mouse strains in the IntelliCage. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9:722–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Lewejohann L, Schwabe K, Häger C, Jirkof P. Impulse for animal welfare outside the experiment. Lab Anim. 2020;54:150–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Baran SW, Bratcher N, Dennis J, Gaburro S, Karlsson EM, Maguire S, et al. Emerging role of translational digital biomarkers within home cage monitoring technologies in preclinical drug discovery and development. Front Behav Neurosci. 2022;15: 758274.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes. ABI Nr. L276/33-L276/79 (last accessed on 11 November 2022). Available under: https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/ALL/?uri=celex%3A32010L0063.

  29. Adams LM, Geyer MA. LSD-induced alterations of locomotor patterns and exploration in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1982;77:179–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Allen DL, Harrison BC, Maass A, Bell ML, Byrnes WC, Leinwand LA. Cardiac and skeletal muscle adaptations to voluntary wheel running in the mouse. J Appl Physiol. 1985;2001(90):1900–8.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Babineau BA, Yang M, Berman RF, Crawley JN. Low home cage social behaviors in BTBR T+tf/J mice during juvenile development. Physiol Behav. 2013;114–115:49–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Beatty WW, Costello KB. Olfactory bulbectomy and play fighting in juvenile rats. Physiol Behav. 1983;30:525–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Breuer ME, McGinnis MY, Lumia AR, Possidente BP. Aggression in male rats receiving anabolic androgenic steroids: effects of social and environmental provocation. Horm Behav. 2001;40:409–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Francis NA, Bohlke K, Kanold PO. Automated behavioral experiments in mice reveal periodic cycles of task engagement within circadian rhythms. eNeuro. 2019;6:ENEURO.0121-19.2019.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Remmelink E, Chau U, Smit AB, Verhage M, Loos M. A one-week 5-choice serial reaction time task to measure impulsivity and attention in adult and adolescent mice. Sci Rep. 2017;7:42519.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Endo T, Maekawa F, Võikar V, Haijima A, Uemura Y, Zhang Y, et al. Automated test of behavioral flexibility in mice using a behavioral sequencing task in IntelliCage. Behav Brain Res. 2011;221:172–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Abbott CR, Small CJ, Sajedi A, Smith KL, Parkinson JR, Broadhead LL, et al. The importance of acclimatisation and habituation to experimental conditions when investigating the anorectic effects of gastrointestinal hormones in the rat. Int J Obes (Lond). 2006;30:288–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Bonasera SJ, Chaudoin TR, Goulding EH, Mittek M, Dunaevsky A. Decreased home cage movement and oromotor impairments in adult Fmr1-KO mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2017;16:564–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Machado TD, Dalle Molle R, Laureano DP, Portella AK, Werlang IC, Benetti Cda S, et al. Early life stress is associated with anxiety, increased stress responsivity and preference for “comfort foods” in adult female rats. Stress. 2013;16:549–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Bohus B. Telemetered heart rate responses of the rat during free and learned behavior. Biotelemetry. 1974;1:193–201.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Brodkin J, Bradbury M, Busse C, Warren N, Bristow LJ, Varney MA. Reduced stress-induced hyperthermia in mGluR5 knockout mice. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16:2241–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Drugan RC, Hibl PT, Kelly KJ, Dady KF, Hale MW, Lowry CA. Prior cold water swim stress alters immobility in the forced swim test and associated activation of serotonergic neurons in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus. Neuroscience. 2013;253:221–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kaneko K, Chikamoto A, Hsu JC, Tochinai R, Sekizawa SI, Yamamoto M, et al. Effects of environmental enrichment on autonomic nervous activity in NSY mice. Exp Anim. 2020;69:161–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Richardson CA. The power of automated behavioural homecage technologies in characterizing disease progression in laboratory mice: a review. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015;163:19–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Maroteaux G, Loos M, van der Sluis S, Koopmans B, Aarts E, van Gassen K, et al. High-throughput phenotyping of avoidance learning in mice discriminates different genotypes and identifies a novel gene. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11:772–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Pernold K, Iannello F, Low BE, Rigamonti M, Rosati G, Scavizzi F, et al. Towards large scale automated cage monitoring - Diurnal rhythm and impact of interventions on in-cage activity of C57BL/6J mice recorded 24/7 with a non-disrupting capacitive-based technique. PLoS ONE. 2019;14: e0211063.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Golini E, Rigamonti M, Iannello F, De Rosa C, Scavizzi F, Raspa M, et al. A non-invasive digital biomarker for the detection of rest disturbances in the SOD1G93A mouse model of ALS. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:896.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Lewejohann L, Hoppmann AM, Kegel P, Kritzler M, Krüger A, Sachser N. Behavioral phenotyping of a murine model of Alzheimer’s disease in a seminaturalistic environment using RFID tracking. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41:850–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Mieske P, Diederich K, Lewejohann L. Roaming in a land of milk and honey: life trajectories and metabolic rate of female inbred mice living in a semi naturalistic environment. Animals (Basel). 2021;11:3002.

  50. Lipp HP. In: Noldus LPJJ, Grieco F, Loijens LWS, Zimmermann PH, editors. Automated behavioral analysis of mice using INTELLICAGE: inter-laboratory comparisons and validation with exploratory behavior and spatial learning. Wageningen: Measuring Behavior; 2005. p. 66–9.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Winslow W, McDonough I, Tallino S, Decker A, Vural AS, Velazquez R. IntelliCage automated behavioral phenotyping reveals behavior deficits in the 3xTg-AD mouse model of Alzheimer’s Disease associated with brain weight. Front Aging Neurosci. 2021;13: 720214.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Habedank A, Urmersbach B, Kahnau P, Lewejohann L. O mouse, where art thou? The Mouse Position Surveillance System (MoPSS)-an RFID-based tracking system. Behav Res Methods. 2022;54:676–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Hobbiesiefken U, Urmersbach B, Jaap A, Diederich K, Lewejohann L. Rating enrichment items by female group-housed laboratory mice in multiple binary choice tests using an RFID-based tracking system. PLoS ONE. 2023;18: e0278709.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. de Chaumont F, Ey E, Torquet N, Lagache T, Dallongeville S, Imbert A, et al. Real-time analysis of the behaviour of groups of mice via a depth-sensing camera and machine learning. Nat Biomed Eng. 2019;3:930–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Arroyo-Araujo M, Graf R, Maco M, van Dam E, Schenker E, Drinkenburg W, et al. Reproducibility via coordinated standardization: a multi-center study in a Shank2 genetic rat model for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Sci Rep. 2019;9:11602.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Robinson L, Spruijt B, Riedel G. Between and within laboratory reliability of mouse behaviour recorded in home-cage and open-field. J Neurosci Methods. 2018;300:10–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Häger C, Keubler LM, Talbot SR, Biernot S, Weegh N, Buchheister S, et al. Running in the wheel: defining individual severity levels in mice. PLoS Biol. 2018;16: e2006159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Weegh N, Füner J, Janke O, Winter Y, Jung C, Struve B, et al. Wheel running behaviour in group-housed female mice indicates disturbed wellbeing due to DSS colitis. Lab Anim. 2020;54:63–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Spivak DI, Kent RE. Ologs: a categorical framework for knowledge representation. PLoS ONE. 2012;7: e24274.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Tecott LH, Nestler EJ. Neurobehavioral assessment in the information age. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:462–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Bornmann L, Haunschild R, Mutz R. Growth rates of modern science: a latent piecewise growth curve approach to model publication numbers from established and new literature databases. Humanit Soc Sci Commun. 2021;8:224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Beery AK, Zucker I. Sex bias in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:565–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Flórez-Vargas O, Brass A, Karystianis G, Bramhall M, Stevens R, Cruickshank S, et al. Bias in the reporting of sex and age in biomedical research on mouse models. Elife. 2016;5: e13615.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Meziane H, Ouagazzal AM, Aubert L, Wietrzych M, Krezel W. Estrous cycle effects on behavior of C57BL/6J and BALB/cByJ female mice: implications for phenotyping strategies. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6:192–200.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Mogil JS, Chanda ML. The case for the inclusion of female subjects in basic science studies of pain. Pain. 2005;117:1–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Becker JB, Prendergast BJ, Liang JW. Female rats are not more variable than male rats: a meta-analysis of neuroscience studies. Biol Sex Differ. 2016;7:34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Fritz AK, Amrein I, Wolfer DP. Similar reliability and equivalent performance of female and male mice in the open field and water-maze place navigation task. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet. 2017;175:380–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Prendergast BJ, Onishi KG, Zucker I. Female mice liberated for inclusion in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;40:1–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Clayton JA, Collins FS. Policy: NIH to balance sex in cell and animal studies. Nature. 2014;509:282–3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Olsson IAS, Westlund K. More than numbers matter: the effect of social factors on behaviour and welfare of laboratory rodents and non-human primates. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2007;103:229–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Chourbaji S, Zacher C, Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R, Gass P. Social and structural housing conditions influence the development of a depressive-like phenotype in the learned helplessness paradigm in male mice. Behav Brain Res. 2005;164:100–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Kalliokoski O, Teilmann AC, Jacobsen KR, Abelson KS, Hau J. The lonely mouse - single housing affects serotonergic signaling integrity measured by 8-OH-DPAT-induced hypothermia in male mice. PLoS ONE. 2014;9: e111065.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Berry A, Bellisario V, Capoccia S, Tirassa P, Calza A, Alleva E, et al. Social deprivation stress is a triggering factor for the emergence of anxiety- and depression-like behaviours and leads to reduced brain BDNF levels in C57BL/6J mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:762–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Bartolomucci A, Palanza P, Sacerdote P, Ceresini G, Chirieleison A, Panerai AE, et al. Individual housing induces altered immuno-endocrine responses to psychological stress in male mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28:540–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Valzelli L. The, “isolation syndrome” in mice. Psychopharmacologia. 1973;31:305–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Sun M, Choi EY, Magee DJ, Stets CW, During MJ, Lin EJ. Metabolic effects of social isolation in adult C57BL/6 mice. Int Sch Res Notices. 2014;2014: 690950.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cacioppo JT. Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychol Bull. 2014;140:1464–504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Huang HJ, Liang KC, Ke HC, Chang YY, Hsieh-Li HM. Long-term social isolation exacerbates the impairment of spatial working memory in APP/PS1 transgenic mice. Brain Res. 2011;1371:150–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Kappel S, Hawkins P, Mendl MT. To group or not to group? Good practice for housing male laboratory mice. Animals (Basel). 2017;7:88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Van Loo PL, Van Zutphen LF, Baumans V. Male management: coping with aggression problems in male laboratory mice. Lab Anim. 2003;37:300–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Weber EM, Dallaire JA, Gaskill BN, Pritchett-Corning KR, Garner JP. Aggression in group-housed laboratory mice: why can’t we solve the problem? Lab Anim (NY). 2017;46:157–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Lidster K, Owen K, Browne WJ, Prescott MJ. Cage aggression in group-housed laboratory male mice: an international data crowdsourcing project. Sci Rep. 2019;9:15211.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Weber EM, Zidar J, Ewaldsson B, Askevik K, Udén E, Svensk E, et al. Aggression in group-housed male mice: a systematic review. Animals. 2023;13:143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Doty RL. Odor-guided behavior in mammals. Experientia. 1986;42:257–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Lim MA, Defensor EB, Mechanic JA, Shah PP, Jaime EA, Roberts CR, et al. Retrospective analysis of the effects of identification procedures and cage changing by using data from automated, continuous monitoring. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2019;58:126–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Febinger HY, George A, Priestley J, Toth LA, Opp MR. Effects of housing condition and cage change on characteristics of sleep in mice. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2014;53:29–37.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Saibaba P, Sales GD, Stodulski G, Hau J. Behaviour of rats in their home cages: daytime variations and effects of routine husbandry procedures analysed by time sampling techniques. Lab Anim. 1996;30:13–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Duke JL, Zammit TG, Lawson DM. The effects of routine cage-changing on cardiovascular and behavioral parameters in male Sprague-Dawley rats. Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci. 2001;40:17–20.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Meller A, Kasanen I, Ruksenas O, Apanaviciene N, Baturaite Z, Voipio HM, et al. Refining cage change routines: comparison of cardiovascular responses to three different ways of cage change in rats. Lab Anim. 2011;45:167–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Rasmussen S, Miller MM, Filipski SB, Tolwani RJ. Cage change influences serum corticosterone and anxiety-like behaviors in the mouse. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2011;50:479–83.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Ghosal S, Nunley A, Mahbod P, Lewis AG, Smith EP, Tong J, et al. Mouse handling limits the impact of stress on metabolic endpoints. Physiol Behav. 2015;150:31–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Freund J, Brandmaier AM, Lewejohann L, Kirste I, Kritzler M, Krüger A, et al. Emergence of individuality in genetically identical mice. Science. 2013;340:756–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Freund J, Brandmaier AM, Lewejohann L, Kirste I, Kritzler M, Krüger A, et al. Association between exploratory activity and social individuality in genetically identical mice living in the same enriched environment. Neuroscience. 2015;309:140–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Lewejohann L, Zipser B, Sachser N. “Personality” in laboratory mice used for biomedical research: a way of understanding variability? Dev Psychobiol. 2011;53:624–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Ripperger JA, Jud C, Albrecht U. The daily rhythm of mice. FEBS Lett. 2011;585:1384–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Eikelboom R, Lattanzio SB. Wheel access duration in rats: II. Day-night and within-session changes Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:825–32.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Burkholder T, Foltz C, Karlsson E, Linton CG, Smith JM. Health evaluation of experimental laboratory mice. Curr Protoc Mouse Biol. 2012;2:145–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Wahlsten D, Metten P, Phillips TJ, Boehm SL 2nd, Burkhart-Kasch S, Dorow J, et al. Different data from different labs: lessons from studies of gene-environment interaction. J Neurobiol. 2003;54:283–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Sorge RE, Martin LJ, Isbester KA, Sotocinal SG, Rosen S, Tuttle AH, et al. Olfactory exposure to males, including men, causes stress and related analgesia in rodents. Nat Methods. 2014;11:629–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. von Kortzfleisch VT, Ambrée O, Karp NA, Meyer N, Novak J, Palme R, et al. Do multiple experimenters improve the reproducibility of animal studies? PLoS Biol. 2022;20: e3001564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Stinus L, Gaffori O, Simon H, Le Moal M. Disappearance of hoarding and disorganization of eating behavior after ventral mesencephalic tegmentum lesions in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1978;92:289–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Young RC, Ervin GN, Smith GP. Abnormal open field behavior after anterolateral hypothalamic injection of 6-hydroxydopamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1976;5:565–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Zwirner PP, Porsolt RD, Loew DM. Inter-group aggression in mice: a new method for testing the effects of centrally active drugs. Psychopharmacologia. 1975;45:133–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Tricklebank MD, Smart JL, Bloxam DL, Curzon G. Effects of chronic experimental liver dysfunction and L-tryptophan on behaviour in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1978;9:181–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Kumstel S, Vasudevan P, Palme R, Zhang X, Wendt EHU, David R, et al. Benefits of non-invasive methods compared to telemetry for distress analysis in a murine model of pancreatic cancer. J Adv Res. 2020;21:35–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Fares R, Flénet T, Vial J, Ravaz M, Roger V, Bory C, et al. Non invasive jacketed telemetry in socially-housed rats for a combined assessment of respiratory system, electrocardiogram and activity using the DECRO system. J Pharmacol Toxicol Methods. 2022;117: 107195.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Bohnslav JP, Wimalasena NK, Clausing KJ, Dai YY, Yarmolinsky DA, Cruz T, et al. DeepEthogram, a machine learning pipeline for supervised behavior classification from raw pixels. Elife. 2021;10: e63377.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  108. Pereira TD, Tabris N, Matsliah A, Turner DM, Li J, Ravindranath S, et al. SLEAP: a deep learning system for multi-animal pose tracking. Nat Methods. 2022;19:486–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Lauer J, Zhou M, Ye S, Menegas W, Schneider S, Nath T, et al. Multi-animal pose estimation, identification and tracking with DeepLabCut. Nat Methods. 2022;19:496–504.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  110. Scells H, Zuccon G. Searchrefiner: a query visualisation and understanding tool for systematic reviews. In: Proceedings of the 27th International CIKM Conference on Information and Knowledge Management. 2018. p. 1939–42.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This article is based upon work from COST Action “Improving biomedical research by automated behaviour monitoring in the animal home-cage” (TEATIME; CA20135; cost-teatime.org) supported by COST (European Cooperation in Science and Technology).

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. VV is supported by Jane and Aatos Erkko Foundation.

PK, PM, AJ, BL, CTR, LL, and KH were funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) under Germany’s Excellence Strategy—EXC 2002/1 “Science of Intelligence” —project number 390523135.

The Vienna BioCenter Core Facilities (VBCF) Preclinical Phenotyping Facility acknowledges funding from the Austrian Federal Ministry of Education, Science & Research; and the City of Vienna.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors read and approved the final manuscript. PK: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; PM: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Investigation, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; JW: Data Curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; OK: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; SM: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; SMH: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; VV: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; AA: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; SB: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; AB: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; AC: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; MC: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; EE: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; EG: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; AJ: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—Review & Editing; DH: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; AK: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; BL: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; NLV: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; HM: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; AR: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; MR: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; MLS: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; NT: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; JT: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; BU: Data Curation, Investigation, Writing—Review & Editing; CTR: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; KD: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; LL: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; KH: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project Administration, Supervision, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katharina Hohlbaum.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

 Objective, Materials & Methods.

Additional file 2.

 Mouse and rat strains (more than one strain could be used in a study; data obtained from n = 521 publications.

Additional file 3.

 Home cage monitoring system (more than one system could be used in a study; data obtained from n = 521 publications).

Additional file 4.

 Social housing structure in single-sex studies (data obtained from n = 187 single-sex studies including mice and n = 191 single-sex studies including rats).

Additional file 5.

 Behavioral, physiological, and external appearance-related parameters (data obtained from n = 521 publications).

Additional file 6.

  Data extraction template.

Additional file 7.

 Data (data obtained from n = 521 publications).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kahnau, P., Mieske, P., Wilzopolski, J. et al. A systematic review of the development and application of home cage monitoring in laboratory mice and rats. BMC Biol 21, 256 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12915-023-01751-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12915-023-01751-7

Keywords